Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • My Account Login
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 10 December 2020

Effect of internet use and electronic game-play on academic performance of Australian children

  • Md Irteja Islam 1 , 2 ,
  • Raaj Kishore Biswas 3 &
  • Rasheda Khanam 1  

Scientific Reports volume  10 , Article number:  21727 ( 2020 ) Cite this article

71k Accesses

23 Citations

32 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Human behaviour
  • Risk factors

This study examined the association of internet use, and electronic game-play with academic performance respectively on weekdays and weekends in Australian children. It also assessed whether addiction tendency to internet and game-play is associated with academic performance. Overall, 1704 children of 11–17-year-olds from young minds matter (YMM), a cross-sectional nationwide survey, were analysed. The generalized linear regression models adjusted for survey weights were applied to investigate the association between internet use, and electronic-gaming with academic performance (measured by NAPLAN–National standard score). About 70% of the sample spent > 2 h/day using the internet and nearly 30% played electronic-games for > 2 h/day. Internet users during weekdays (> 4 h/day) were less likely to get higher scores in reading and numeracy, and internet use on weekends (> 2–4 h/day) was positively associated with academic performance. In contrast, 16% of electronic gamers were more likely to get better reading scores on weekdays compared to those who did not. Addiction tendency to internet and electronic-gaming is found to be adversely associated with academic achievement. Further, results indicated the need for parental monitoring and/or self-regulation to limit the timing and duration of internet use/electronic-gaming to overcome the detrimental effects of internet use and electronic game-play on academic achievement.

Similar content being viewed by others

video games affect academic performance essay

Microdosing with psilocybin mushrooms: a double-blind placebo-controlled study

video games affect academic performance essay

What colour are your eyes? Teaching the genetics of eye colour & colour vision. Edridge Green Lecture RCOphth Annual Congress Glasgow May 2019

video games affect academic performance essay

The development of human causal learning and reasoning

Introduction.

Over the past two decades, with the proliferation of high-tech devices (e.g. Smartphone, tablets and computers), both the internet and electronic games have become increasingly popular with people of all ages, but particularly with children and adolescents 1 , 2 , 3 . Recent estimates have shown that one in three under-18-year-olds across the world uses the Internet, and 75% of adolescents play electronic games daily in developed countries 4 , 5 , 6 . Studies in the United States reported that adolescents are occupied with over 11 h a day with modern electronic media such as computer/Internet and electronic games, which is more than they spend in school or with friends 7 , 8 . In Australia, it is reported that about 98% of children aged 15–17 years are among Internet users and 98% of adolescents play electronic games, which is significantly higher than the USA and Europe 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 .

In recent times, the Internet and electronic games have been regarded as important, not just for better results at school, but also for self-expression, sociability, creativity and entertainment for children and adolescents 13 , 14 . For instance, 88% of 12–17 year-olds in the USA considered the Internet as a useful mechanism for making progress in school 15 , and similarly, electronic gaming in children and adolescents may assist in developing skills such as decision-making, smart-thinking and coordination 3 , 15 .

On the other hand, evidence points to the fact that the use of the Internet and electronic games is found to have detrimental effects such as reduced sleeping time, behavioural problems (e.g. low self-esteem, anxiety, depression), attention problems and poor academic performance in adolescents 1 , 5 , 12 , 16 . In addition, excessive Internet usage and increased electronic gaming are found to be addictive and may cause serious functional impairment in the daily life of children and adolescents 1 , 12 , 13 , 16 . For example, the AU Kids Online survey 17 reported that 50% of Australian children were more likely to experience behavioural problems associated with Internet use compared to children from 25 European countries (29%) surveyed in the EU Kids Online study 18 , which is alarming 12 . These mixed results require an urgent need of understanding the effect of the Internet use and electronic gaming on the development of children and adolescents, particularly on their academic performance.

Despite many international studies and a smaller number in Australia 12 , several systematic limitations remain in the existing literature, particularly regarding the association of academic performance with the use of Internet and electronic games in children and adolescents 13 , 16 , 19 . First, the majority of the earlier studies have either relied on school grades or children’s self assessments—which contain an innate subjectivity by the assessor; and have not considered the standardized tests of academic performance 16 , 20 , 21 , 22 . Second, most previous studies have tested the hypothesis in the school-based settings instead of canvassing the whole community, and cannot therefore adjust for sociodemographic confounders 9 , 16 . Third, most studies have been typically limited to smaller sample sizes, which might have reduced the reliability of the results 9 , 16 , 23 .

By considering these issues, this study aimed to investigate the association of internet usage and electronic gaming on a standardized test of academic performance—NAPLAN (The National Assessment Program—Literacy and Numeracy) among Australian adolescents aged 11–17 years using nationally representative data from the Second Australian Child and Adolescent Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing—Young Minds Matter (YMM). It is hypothesized that the findings of this study will provide a population-wide, contextual view of excessive Internet use and electronic games played separately on weekdays and weekends by Australian adolescents, which may be beneficial for evidence-based policies.

Subject demographics

Respondents who attended gave NAPLAN in 2008 (N = 4) and 2009 (N = 29) were removed from the sample due to smaller sample size, as later years (2010–2015) had over 100 samples yearly. The NAPLAN scores from 2008 might not align with a survey conducted in 2013. Further missing cases were deleted with the assumption that data were missing at random for unbiased estimates, which is common for large-scale surveys 24 . From the initial survey of 2967 samples, 1704 adolescents were sampled for this study.

The sample characteristics were displayed in Table 1 . For example, distribution of daily average internet use was checked, showing that over 50% of the sampled adolescents spent 2–4 h on internet (Table 1 ). Although all respondents in the survey used internet, nearly 21% of them did not play any electronic games in a day and almost one in every three (33%) adolescents played electronic games beyond the recommended time of 2 h per day. Girls had more addictive tendency to internet/game-play in compare to boys.

The mean scores for the three NAPLAN tests scores (reading, writing and numeracy) ranged from 520 to 600. A gradual decline in average NAPLAN tests scores (reading, writing and numeracy) scores were observed for internet use over 4 h during weekdays, and over 3 h during weekends (Table 2 ). Table 2 also shows that adolescents who played no electronic games at all have better scores in writing compared to those who play electronic games. Moreover, Table 2 shows no particular pattern between time spent on gaming and NAPLAN reading and numeracy scores. Among the survey samples, 308 adolescents were below the national standard average.

Internet use and academic performance

Our results show that internet (non-academic use) use during weekdays, especially more than 4 h, is negatively associated with academic performance (Table 3 ). For internet use during weekdays, all three models showed a significant negative association between time spent on internet and NAPLAN reading and numeracy scores. For example, in Model 1, adolescents who spent over 4 h on internet during weekdays are 15% and 17% less likely to get higher reading and numeracy scores respectively compared to those who spend less than 2 h. Similar results were found in Model 2 and 3 (Table 3 ), when we adjusted other confounders. The variable addiction tendency to internet was found to be negatively associated with NAPLAN results. The adolescents who had internet addiction were 17% less and 14% less likely to score higher in reading and numeracy respectively than those without such problematic behaviour.

Internet use during weekends showed a positive association with academic performance (Table 4 ). For example, Model 1 in Table 4 shows that internet use during weekends was significant for reading, writing and national standard scores. Youths who spend around 2–4 h and over 4 h on the internet during weekends were 21% and 15% more likely to get a higher reading scores respectively compared to those who spend less than 2 h (Model 1, Table 4 ). Similarly, in model 3, where the internet addiction of adolescents was adjusted, adolescents who spent 2–4 h on internet were 1.59 times more likely to score above the national standard. All three models of Table 4 confirmed that adolescents who spent 2–4 h on the internet during weekends are more likely to achieve better reading and writing scores and be at or above national standard compared to those who used the internet for less than 2 h. Numeracy scores were unlikely to be affected by internet use. The results obtained from Model 3 should be treated as robust, as this is the most comprehensive model that accounts for unobserved characteristics. The addiction tendency to internet/game-play variable showed a negative association with academic performance, but this is only significant for numeracy scores.

Electronic gaming and academic performance

Time spent on electronic gaming during weekdays had no effect on the academic performance of writing and language but had significant association with reading scores (Model 2, Table 5 ). Model 2 of Table 5 shows that adolescents who spent 1–2 h on gaming during weekdays were 13% more likely to get higher reading scores compared to those who did not play at all. It was an interesting result that while electronic gaming during weekdays tended to show a positive effect on reading scores, internet use during weekdays showed a negative effect. Addiction tendency to internet/game-play had a negative effect; the adolescents who were addicted to the internet were 14% less likely to score more highly in reading than those without any such behaviour.

All three models from Table 6 confirm that time spent on electronic gaming over 2 h during weekends had a positive effect on readings scores. For example, the results of Model 3 (Table 6 ) showed that adolescents who spent more than 2 h on electronic gaming during weekdays were 16% more likely to have better reading scores compared to adolescents who did not play games at all. Playing electronic games during weekends was not found to be statistically significant for writing and numeracy scores and national standard scores, although the odds ratios were positive. The results from all tables confirm that addiction tendency to internet/gaming is negatively associated with academic performance, although the variable is not always statistically significant.

Building on past research on the effect of the internet use and electronic gaming in adolescents, this study examined whether Internet use and playing electronic games were associated with academic performance (i.e. reading, writing and numeracy) using a standardized test of academic performance (i.e. NAPLAN) in a nationally representative dataset in Australia. The findings of this study question the conventional belief 9 , 25 that academic performance is negatively associated with internet use and electronic games, particularly when the internet is used for non-academic purpose.

In the current hi-tech world, many developed countries (e.g. the USA, Canada and Australia) have recommended that 5–17 year-olds limit electronic media (e.g. internet, electronic games) to 2 h per day for entertainment purposes, with concerns about the possible negative consequences of excessive use of electronic media 14 , 26 . However, previous research has often reported that children and adolescents spent more than the recommended time 26 . The present study also found similar results, that is, that about 70% of the sampled adolescents aged 11–17 spent more than 2 h per day on the Internet and nearly 30% spent more than 2-h on electronic gaming in a day. This could be attributed to the increased availability of computers/smart-phones and the internet among under-18s 12 . For instance, 97% of Australian households with children aged less than 15 years accessed internet at home in 2016–2017 10 ; as a result, policymakers recommended that parents restrict access to screens (e.g. Internet and electronic games) in children’s bedrooms, monitor children using screens, share screen hours with their children, and to act as role models by reducing their own screen time 14 .

This research has drawn attention to the fact that the average time spent using the internet, which is often more than 4 h during weekdays tends to be negatively associated with academic performance, especially a lower reading and numeracy score, while internet use of more than 2 h during weekends is positively associated with academic performance, particularly having a better reading and writing score and above national standard score. By dividing internet use and gaming by weekdays and weekends, this study find an answer to the mixed evidence found in previous literature 9 . The results of this study clearly show that the non-academic use of internet during weekdays, particularly, spending more than 4 h on internet is harmful for academic performance, whereas, internet use on the weekends is likely to incur a positive effect on academic performance. This result is consistent with a USA study that reported that internet use is positively associated with improved reading skills and higher scores on standardized tests 13 , 27 . It is also reported in the literature that academic performance is better among moderate users of the internet compared to non-users or high level users 13 , 27 , which was in line with the findings of this study. This may be due to the fact that the internet is predominantly a text-based format in which the internet users need to type and read to access most websites effectively 13 . The results of this study indicated that internet use is not harmful to academic performance if it is used moderately, especially, if ensuring very limited use on weekdays. The results of this study further confirmed that timing (weekdays or weekends) of internet use is a factor that needs to be considered.

Regarding electronic gaming, interestingly, the study found that the average time of gaming either in weekdays or weekends is positively associated with academic performance especially for reading scores. These results contradicted previous literatures 1 , 13 , 19 , 27 that have reported negative correlation between electronic games and educational performance in high-school children. The results of this study were consistent with studies conducted in the USA, Europe and other countries that claimed a positive correlation between gaming and academic performance, especially in numeracy and reading skills 28 , 29 . This is may be due to the fact that the instructions for playing most of the electronic games are text-heavy and many electronic games require gamers to solve puzzles 9 , 30 . The literature also found that playing electronic games develops cognitive skills (e.g. mental rotation abilities, dexterity), which can be attributable to better academic achievement 31 , 32 .

Consistent with previous research findings 33 , 34 , 35 , 36 , the study also found that adolescents who had addiction tendency to internet usage and/or electronic gaming were less likely to achieve higher scores in reading and numeracy compared to those who had not problematic behaviour. Addiction tendency to Internet/gaming among adolescents was found to be negatively associated with overall academic performance compared to those who were not having addiction tendency, although the variables were not always statistically significant. This is mainly because adolescents’ skipped school and missed classes and tuitions, and provide less effort to do homework due to addictive internet usage and electronic gaming 19 , 35 . The results of this study indicated that parental monitoring and/ or self-regulation (by the users) regarding the timing and intensity of internet use/gaming are essential to outweigh any negative effect of internet use and gaming on academic performance.

Although the present study uses a large nationally representative sample and advances prior research on the academic performance among adolescents who reported using the internet and playing electronic games, the findings of this study also have some limitations that need to be addressed. Firstly, adolescents who reported on the internet use and electronic games relied on self-reported child data without any screening tests or any external validation and thus, results may be overestimated or underestimated. Second, the study primarily addresses the internet use and electronic games as distinct behaviours, as the YMM survey gathered information only on the amount of time spent on internet use and electronic gaming, and included only a few questions related to addiction due to resources and time constraints and did not provide enough information to medically diagnose internet/gaming addiction. Finally, the cross-sectional research design of the data outlawed evaluation of causality and temporality of the observed association of internet use and electronic gaming with the academic performance in adolescents.

This study found that the average time spent on the internet on weekends and electronic gaming (both in weekdays and weekends) is positively associated with academic performance (measured by NAPLAN) of Australian adolescents. However, it confirmed a negative association between addiction tendency (internet use or electronic gaming) and academic performance; nonetheless, most of the adolescents used the internet and played electronic games more than the recommended 2-h limit per day. The study also revealed that further research is required on the development and implementation of interventions aimed at improving parental monitoring and fostering users’ self-regulation to restrict the daily usage of the internet and/or electronic games.

Data description

Young minds matter (YMM) was an Australian nationwide cross-sectional survey, on children aged 4–17 years conducted in 2013–2014 37 . Out of the initial 76,606 households approached, a total of 6,310 parents/caregivers (eligible household response rate 55%) of 4–17 year-old children completed a structured questionnaire via face to face interview and 2967 children aged 11–17 years (eligible children response rate 89%) completed a computer-based self-reported questionnaire privately at home 37 .

Area based sampling was used for the survey. A total of 225 Statistical Area 1 (defined by Australian Bureau of Statistics) areas were selected based on the 2011 Census of Population and Housing. They were stratified by state/territory and by metropolitan versus non-metropolitan (rural/regional) to ensure proportional representation of geographic areas across Australia 38 . However, a small number of samples were excluded, based on most remote areas, homeless children, institutional care and children living in households where interviews could not be conducted in English. The details of the survey and methodology used in the survey can be found in Lawrence et al. 37 .

Following informed consent (both written and verbal) from the primary carers (parents/caregivers), information on the National Assessment Program—Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN) of the children and adolescents were also added to the YMM dataset. The YMM survey is ethically approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Western Australia and by the Australian Government Department of Health. In addition, the authors of this study obtained a written approval from Australian Data Archive (ADA) Dataverse to access the YMM dataset. All the researches were done in accordance with relevant ADA Dataverse guidelines and policy/regulations in using YMM datasets.

Outcome variables

The NAPLAN, conducted annually since 2008, is a nationwide standardized test of academic performance for all Australian students in Years 3, 5, 7 and 9 to assess their skills in reading, writing numeracy, grammar and spelling 39 , 40 . NAPLAN scores from 2010 to 2015, reported by YMM, were used as outcome variables in the models; while NAPLAN data of 2008 (N = 4) and 2009 (N = 29) were excluded for this study in order to reduce the time lag between YMM survey and the NAPLAN test. The NAPLAN gives point-in-time standardized scores, which provide the scope to compare children’s academic performance over time 40 , 41 . The NAPLAN tests are one component of the evaluation and grading phase of each school, and do not substitute for the comprehensive, consistent evaluations provided by teachers on the performance of each student 39 , 41 . All four domains—reading, writing, numeracy and language conventions (grammar and spelling) are in continuous scales in the dataset. The scores are given based on a series of tests; details can be found in 42 . The current study uses only reading, writing and numeracy scores to measure academic performance.

In this study, the National standard score is a combination of three variables: whether the student meets the national standard in reading, writing and numeracy. Based on national average score, a binary outcome variable is also generated. One category is ‘below standard’ if a child scores at least one standard deviation (one below scores) from the national standard in reading, writing and numeracy, and the rest is ‘at/above standard’.

Independent variables

Internet use and electronic gaming.

In the YMM survey, owing to the scope of the survey itself, an extensive set of questions about internet usage and electronic gaming could not be included. Internet usage omitted the time spent in academic purposes and/or related activities. Playing electronic games included playing games on a gaming console (e.g. PlayStation, Xbox, or similar console ) online or using a computer, or mobile phone, or a handled device 12 . The primary independent covariates were average internet use per day and average electronic game-play in hours per day. A combination of hours on weekdays and weekends was separately used in the models. These variables were based on a self-assessed questionnaire where the youths were asked questions regarding daily time spent on the Internet and electronic game-play, specifically on either weekends or weekdays. Since, internet use/game-play for a maximum of 2 h/day is recommended for children and adolescents aged between 5 and 17 years in many developed countries including Australia 14 , 26 ; therefore, to be consistent with the recommended time we preferred to categorize both the time variables of internet use and gaming into three groups with an interval of 2 h each. Internet use was categorized into three groups: (a) ≤ 2 h), (b) 2–4 h, and (c) > 4 h. Similar questions were asked for game-play h. The sample distribution for electronic game-play was skewed; therefore, this variable was categorized into three groups: (a) no game-play (0 h), (b) 1–2 h, and (c) > 2 h.

Other covariates

Family structure and several sociodemographic variables were used in the models to adjust for the differences in individual characteristics, parental inputs and tastes, household characteristics and place of residence. Individual characteristics included age (continuous) and sex of the child (boys, girls) and addiction tendency to internet use and/or game-play of the adolescent. Addiction tendency to internet/game-play was a binary independent variable. It was a combination of five behavioural questions relating to: whether the respondent avoided eating/sleeping due to internet use or game-play; feels bothered when s/he cannot access internet or play electronic games; keeps using internet or playing electronic games even when s/he is not really interested; spends less time with family/friends or on school works due to internet use or game-play; and unsuccessfully tries to spend less time on the internet or playing electronic games. There were four options for each question: never/almost never; not very often; fairly often; and very often. A binary covariate was simulated, where if any four out of five behaviours were reported as for example, fairly often or very often, then it was considered that the respondent had addictive tendency.

Household characteristics included household income (low, medium, high), family type (original, step, blended, sole parent/primary carer, other) 43 and remoteness (major cities, inner regional, outer regional, remote/very remote). Parental inputs and taste included education of primary carer (bachelor, diploma, year 10/11), primary carer’s likelihood of serious mental illness (K6 score -likely; not likely); primary carer’s smoking status (no, yes); and risk of alcoholic related harm by the primary carer (risky, none).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics of the sample and distributions of the outcome variables were initially assessed. Based on these distributions, the categorization of outcome variables was conducted, as mentioned above. For formal analysis, generalized linear regression models (GLMs) 44 were used, adjusting for the survey weights, which allowed for generalization of the findings. As NAPLAN scores of three areas—reading, writing and numeracy—were continuous variables, linear models were fitted to daily average internet time and electronic game play time. The scores were standardized (mean = 0, SD = 1) for model fitness. The binary logistic model was fitted for the dichotomized national standard outcome variable. Separate models were estimated for internet and electronic gaming on weekends and weekdays.

We estimated three different models, where models varied based on covariates used to adjust the GLMs. Model 1 was adjusted for common sociodemographic factors including age and sex of the child, household income, education of primary carer’s and family type 43 . However, the results of this model did not account for some unobserved household characteristics (e.g. taste, preferences) that are unobserved to the researcher and are arguably correlated with potential outcomes. The effects of unobserved characteristics were reduced by using a comprehensive set of observable characteristics 45 , 46 that were available in YMM data. The issue of unobserved characteristics was addressed by estimating two additional models that include variables by including household characteristics such as parental taste, preference and inputs, and child characteristics in the model. In addition to the variables in Model 1, Model 2 included remoteness, primary carer’s mental health status, smoking status and risk of alcoholic related harm by the primary carer. Model 3 further included internet/game addiction of the adolescent in addition to all the covariates in Model 2. Model 3 was expected to account for a child’s level of unobserved characteristics as the children who were addicted to internet/games were different from others. The model will further show how academic performance is affected by internet/game addiction. The correlation among the variables ‘internet/game addiction’ and ‘internet use’ and ‘gaming’ (during weekdays and weekends) were also assessed, and they were less than 0.5. Multicollinearity was assessed using the variance inflation factor (VIF), which was under 5 for all models, suggesting no multicollinearity 47 .

p value below the threshold of 0.05 was considered the threshold of significance. All analysis was conducted in R (version 3.6.1). R-package survey (version 3.37) was used for modelling which is suited for complex survey samples 48 .

Data availability

The authors declare that they do not have permission to share dataset. However, the datasets of Young Minds Matter (YMM) survey data is available at the Australian Data Archive (ADA) Dataverse on request ( https://doi.org/10.4225/87/LCVEU3 ).

Wang, C. -W., Chan, C. L., Mak, K. -K., Ho, S. -Y., Wong, P. W. & Ho, R. T. Prevalence and correlates of video and Internet gaming addiction among Hong Kong adolescents: a pilot study. Sci. World J . 2014 (2014).

Anderson, E. L., Steen, E. & Stavropoulos, V. Internet use and problematic internet use: a systematic review of longitudinal research trends in adolescence and emergent adulthood. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 22 , 430–454 (2017).

Article   Google Scholar  

Oliveira, M. P. MTd. et al. Use of internet and electronic games by adolescents at high social risk. Trends Psychol. 25 , 1167–1183 (2017).

Google Scholar  

UNICEF. Children in a digital world. United Nations Children's Fund (UNICEF) (2017)

King, D. L. et al. The impact of prolonged violent video-gaming on adolescent sleep: an experimental study. J. Sleep Res. 22 , 137–143 (2013).

Byrne, J. & Burton, P. Children as Internet users: how can evidence better inform policy debate?. J. Cyber Policy. 2 , 39–52 (2017).

Council, O. Children, adolescents, and the media. Pediatrics 132 , 958 (2013).

Paulus, F. W., Ohmann, S., Von Gontard, A. & Popow, C. Internet gaming disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 60 , 645–659 (2018).

Posso, A. Internet usage and educational outcomes among 15-year old Australian students. Int J Commun 10 , 26 (2016).

ABS. 8146.0—Household Use of Information Technology, Australia, 2016–2017 (2018).

Brand, J. E. Digital Australia 2018 (Interactive Games & Entertainment Association (IGEA), Eveleigh, 2017).

Rikkers, W., Lawrence, D., Hafekost, J. & Zubrick, S. R. Internet use and electronic gaming by children and adolescents with emotional and behavioural problems in Australia–results from the second Child and Adolescent Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. BMC Public Health 16 , 399 (2016).

Jackson, L. A., Von Eye, A., Witt, E. A., Zhao, Y. & Fitzgerald, H. E. A longitudinal study of the effects of Internet use and videogame playing on academic performance and the roles of gender, race and income in these relationships. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27 , 228–239 (2011).

Yu, M. & Baxter, J. Australian children’s screen time and participation in extracurricular activities. Ann. Stat. Rep. 2016 , 99 (2015).

Rainie, L. & Horrigan, J. A decade of adoption: How the Internet has woven itself into American life. Pew Internet and American Life Project . 25 (2005).

Drummond, A. & Sauer, J. D. Video-games do not negatively impact adolescent academic performance in science, mathematics or reading. PLoS ONE 9 , e87943 (2014).

Article   ADS   CAS   Google Scholar  

Green, L., Olafsson, K., Brady, D. & Smahel, D. Excessive Internet use among Australian children (2012).

Livingstone, S. EU kids online. The international encyclopedia of media literacy . 1–17 (2019).

Wright, J. The effects of video game play on academic performance. Mod. Psychol. Stud. 17 , 6 (2011).

Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R. & Walsh, D. A. The effects of violent video game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school performance. J. Adolesc. 27 , 5–22 (2004).

Rosenthal, R. & Jacobson, L. Pygmalion in the classroom. Urban Rev. 3 , 16–20 (1968).

Willoughby, T. A short-term longitudinal study of Internet and computer game use by adolescent boys and girls: prevalence, frequency of use, and psychosocial predictors. Dev. Psychol. 44 , 195 (2008).

Weis, R. & Cerankosky, B. C. Effects of video-game ownership on young boys’ academic and behavioral functioning: a randomized, controlled study. Psychol. Sci. 21 , 463–470 (2010).

Howell, D. C. The treatment of missing data. The Sage handbook of social science methodology . 208–224 (2007).

Terry, M. and Malik, A. Video gaming as a factor that affects academic performance in grade nine. Online Submission (2018).

Houghton, S. et al. Virtually impossible: limiting Australian children and adolescents daily screen based media use. BMC Public Health. 15 , 5 (2015).

Jackson, L. A., Von Eye, A., Fitzgerald, H. E., Witt, E. A. & Zhao, Y. Internet use, videogame playing and cell phone use as predictors of children’s body mass index (BMI), body weight, academic performance, and social and overall self-esteem. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27 , 599–604 (2011).

Bowers, A. J. & Berland, M. Does recreational computer use affect high school achievement?. Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 61 , 51–69 (2013).

Wittwer, J. & Senkbeil, M. Is students’ computer use at home related to their mathematical performance at school?. Comput. Educ. 50 , 1558–1571 (2008).

Jackson, L. A. et al. Does home internet use influence the academic performance of low-income children?. Dev. Psychol. 42 , 429 (2006).

Barlett, C. P., Anderson, C. A. & Swing, E. L. Video game effects—confirmed, suspected, and speculative: a review of the evidence. Simul. Gaming 40 , 377–403 (2009).

Suziedelyte, A. Can video games affect children's cognitive and non-cognitive skills? UNSW Australian School of Business Research Paper (2012).

Chiu, S.-I., Lee, J.-Z. & Huang, D.-H. Video game addiction in children and teenagers in Taiwan. CyberPsychol. Behav. 7 , 571–581 (2004).

Skoric, M. M., Teo, L. L. C. & Neo, R. L. Children and video games: addiction, engagement, and scholastic achievement. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12 , 567–572 (2009).

Leung, L. & Lee, P. S. Impact of internet literacy, internet addiction symptoms, and internet activities on academic performance. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 30 , 403–418 (2012).

Xin, M. et al. Online activities, prevalence of Internet addiction and risk factors related to family and school among adolescents in China. Addict. Behav. Rep. 7 , 14–18 (2018).

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Lawrence, D., Johnson, S., Hafekost, J., et al. The mental health of children and adolescents: report on the second Australian child and adolescent survey of mental health and wellbeing (2015).

Hafekost, J. et al. Methodology of young minds matter: the second Australian child and adolescent survey of mental health and wellbeing. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 50 , 866–875 (2016).

Australian Curriculum ARAA. National Assessment Program Literacy and Numeracy: Achievement in Reading, Persuasive Writing, Language Conventions and Numeracy: National Report for 2011 . Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority (2011).

Daraganova, G., Edwards, B. & Sipthorp, M. Using National Assessment Program Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN) Data in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC) . Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (2013).

NAP. NAPLAN (2016).

Australian Curriculum ARAA. National report on schooling in Australia 2009. Ministerial Council for Education, Early Childhood Development and Youth… (2009).

Vu, X.-B.B., Biswas, R. K., Khanam, R. & Rahman, M. Mental health service use in Australia: the role of family structure and socio-economic status. Children Youth Serv. Rev. 93 , 378–389 (2018).

McCullagh, P. Generalized Linear Models (Routledge, Abingdon, 2019).

Book   Google Scholar  

Gregg, P., Washbrook, E., Propper, C. & Burgess, S. The effects of a mother’s return to work decision on child development in the UK. Econ. J. 115 , F48–F80 (2005).

Khanam, R. & Nghiem, S. Family income and child cognitive and noncognitive development in Australia: does money matter?. Demography 53 , 597–621 (2016).

Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., Neter, J. & Li, W. Applied Linear Statistical Models (McGraw-Hill Irwin, New York, 2005).

Lumley T. Package ‘survey’. 3 , 30–33 (2015).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the University of Western Australia, Roy Morgan Research, the Australian Government Department of Health for conducting the survey, and the Australian Data Archive for giving access to the YMM survey dataset. The authors also would like to thank Dr Barbara Harmes for proofreading the manuscript.

This research did not receive any specific Grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Centre for Health Research and School of Commerce, University of Southern Queensland, Workstation 15, Room T450, Block T, Toowoomba, QLD, 4350, Australia

Md Irteja Islam & Rasheda Khanam

Maternal and Child Health Division, International Centre for Diarrhoeal Disease Research, Bangladesh (icddr,b), Mohakhali, Dhaka, 1212, Bangladesh

Md Irteja Islam

Transport and Road Safety (TARS) Research Centre, School of Aviation, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, 2052, Australia

Raaj Kishore Biswas

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

M.I.I.: Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Investigation, Writing—Original draft preparation, Writing—Reviewing and Editing. R.K.B.: Methodology, Software, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Writing—Original draft preparation. R.K.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Supervision, Writing- Reviewing and Editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Md Irteja Islam .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Islam, M.I., Biswas, R.K. & Khanam, R. Effect of internet use and electronic game-play on academic performance of Australian children. Sci Rep 10 , 21727 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78916-9

Download citation

Received : 28 August 2020

Accepted : 02 December 2020

Published : 10 December 2020

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78916-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

I want to play a game: examining sex differences in the effects of pathological gaming, academic self-efficacy, and academic initiative on academic performance in adolescence.

  • Sara Madeleine Kristensen
  • Magnus Jørgensen

Education and Information Technologies (2024)

Measurement Invariance of the Lemmens Internet Gaming Disorder Scale-9 Across Age, Gender, and Respondents

  • Iulia Maria Coșa
  • Anca Dobrean
  • Robert Balazsi

Psychiatric Quarterly (2024)

Academic and Social Behaviour Profile of the Primary School Students who Possess and Play Video Games

  • E. Vázquez-Cano
  • J. M. Ramírez-Hurtado
  • C. Pascual-Moscoso

Child Indicators Research (2023)

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines . If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

video games affect academic performance essay

The impact of time devoted to video games on student achievement

  • Published: 05 November 2022
  • Volume 28 , pages 5921–5944, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

video games affect academic performance essay

  • Tijana Savić Tot 1 ,
  • Slobodan Adžić 1 ,
  • Vilmoš Tot 2 ,
  • Maja Aleksić 3 &
  • Nebojša Zakić 4  

1702 Accesses

2 Citations

Explore all metrics

The aim of this paper is to examine the relationship between the time that higher education students spend playing video games during exam periods and their average grades in one Eastern European country. Moreover, the authors wanted to explore the differences among students with regard to their age, gender, year of study, and employment status in relation to video game-playing habits. Four research questions were generated and a quantitative survey among students ( N  =  233 ) was conducted at two universities in Serbia in December 2021. The results showed that, on average, students who play more games may have slightly lower grades than students who do not play games. In contrast, the time devoted to studying during the exam period is to some extent related to students’ average achievement. In research sample, the best students spend the least time playing video games and the most time studying compared to other students.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

video games affect academic performance essay

Similar content being viewed by others

video games affect academic performance essay

Effects of Game-Based Learning on Academic Performance and Student Interest

video games affect academic performance essay

The Effect of Game-Based Learning on Student Achievement: A Meta-Analysis Study

video games affect academic performance essay

Motivation Outcomes in Math-Related Videogames

Academic Grading in Serbia. (2022). Academic Dictionaries and Encyclopedias.  https://en-academic.com/dic.nsf/enwiki/10161485 . Accessed 5 May 2022 

ACHA. (2019). National College Health Assessment Data .  https://www.mnsu.edu/university-life/health-and-safety/student-health-services/health-education/national-college-health-assessment-data/ . Accessed 28 Sept 2022

Adachi, P. J. C., & Willoughby, T. (2013). More than just fun and games: The longitudinal relationships between strategic video games, self-reported problem solving skills, and academic grades. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42 (7), 1041–1052. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9913-9

Article   Google Scholar  

Adžić, S., Al-Mansour, J., Naqvi, H., & Stambolić, S. (2021). The impact of video games on Students’ educational outcomes. Entertainment Computing, 38 , 100412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.entcom.2021.100412

Ahmad, S., Batool, A., & Ch, A. H. (2020). Path relationship of time management and academic achievement of students in distance learning institutions. Pakistan Journal of Distance and Online Learning , 5 (2), 191–208. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1266710

Alani, F., Khan, F. R., & Hawas, A. (2020). Do Time Management Factors Impede Students’ Academic Achievement?: A Case-study – Sohar University, Oman (SSRN Scholarly Paper No. 3667477). Social Science Research Network. https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3667477

Altintas, E., Karaca, Y., Hullaert, T., & Tassi, P. (2019). Sleep quality and video game playing: Effect of intensity of video game playing and mental health. Psychiatry Research, 273 , 487–492. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.01.030

Al-Zoubi, S. M., & Younes, M. A. B. (2015). Low academic achievement: Causes and results. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 5 (11), 2262.

Anand, V. (2007). A study of time management: The correlation between video game usage and academic performance markers. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10 (4), 552–559. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.9991

Anderson, C. G., Campbell, K., & Steinkuehler, C. (2019). Building persistence through failure: The role of challenge in video games. Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on the Foundations of Digital Games , 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1145/3337722.3337741

Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., Rothstein, H. R., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136 (2), 151. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018251

Barr, M. (2020). A cross-sectional study of video game play habits and graduate skills attainment. Research in Learning Technology, 28 , 2326. https://doi.org/10.25304/rlt.v28.2326

Bateman, C., & Boon, R. (2005). 21st Century Game Design (Game Development Series) . Charles River Media, Inc.

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21 (4), 327–336.

Bewick, B., Koutsopoulou, G., Miles, J., Slaa, E., & Barkham, M. (2010). Changes in undergraduate students’ psychological well-being as they progress through university. Studies in Higher Education, 35 (6), 633–645.

Bloemer, W., Day, S., & Swan, K. (2017). Gap analysis: An innovative look at gateway courses and student retention. Online Learning, 21 (3), 5–14. https://doi.org/10.24059/olj.v21i3.1233

Borgonovi, F. (2016). Video gaming and gender differences in digital and printed reading performance among 15-year-olds students in 26 countries. Journal of Adolescence , 48 , 45–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.01.004

Bowman, N. D., & Tamborini, R. (2015). “In the Mood to Game”: Selective exposure and mood management processes in computer game play. New Media & Society, 17 (3), 375–393. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444813504274

Burgess, S. R., Stermer, S. P., & Burgess, M. C. R. (2012). Video game playing and academic performance in college students. College Student Journal, 46 (2), 376–388.

Google Scholar  

Cain, M., Prinzmetal, W., Shimamura, A., & Landau, A. (2014). Improved control of exogenous attention in action video game players. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 ,. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00069

Castro-Alonso, J. C., & Fiorella, L. (2019). Interactive science multimedia and visuospatial processing. In Visuospatial processing for education in health and natural sciences (pp. 145–173). Springer.

Chan, P. A., & Rabinowitz, T. (2006). A cross-sectional analysis of video games and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adolescents. Annals of General Psychiatry, 5 (1), 1–10.

Clement, J. (2021). Video game industry—Statistics & Facts . Statista. https://www.statista.com/topics/868/video-games/ . Accessed 13 May 2022

Dobrowolski, P., Hanusz, K., Sobczyk, B., Skorko, M., & Wiatrow, A. (2015). Cognitive enhancement in video game players: The role of video game genre. Computers in Human Behavior, 44 , 59–63.

Domingues-Montanari, S. (2017). Clinical and psychological effects of excessive screen time on children. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 53 , 333–338. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpc.13462

Donohue, S. E., Woldorff, M. G., & Mitroff, S. R. (2010). Video game players show more precise multisensory temporal processing abilities. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72 , 1120–1129. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.4.1120

Drummond, A., & Sauer, J. D. (2014). Video-games do not negatively impact adolescent academic performance in science, mathematics or reading. PLOS ONE, 9 (4), e87943. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087943

Drummond, A., & Sauer, J. D. (2020). Timesplitters: Playing video games before (but not after) school on weekdays is associated with poorer adolescent academic performance. A test of competing theoretical accounts. Computers & Education, 144 , 103704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103704

Entertainment Software Association, E. (2021). Essential Facts About the Video Game Industry . Entertainment Software Association.  https://www.theesa.com/resource/2021-essential-facts-about-the-video-game-industry/ . Accessed 16 March 2022

Entertainment Software Association ESA. (2021). Featured Stories About Video game Industry . Entertainment Software Association.  https://www.theesa.com/ . Accessed 13 May 2022

Eskin, M., Sun, J.-M., Abuidhail, J., Yoshimasu, K., Kujan, O., Janghorbani, M., Flood, C., Carta, M. G., Tran, U. S., & Mechri, A. (2016). Suicidal behavior and psychological distress in university students: A 12-nation study. Archives of Suicide Research, 20 (3), 369–388.

Feldman, D. B., & Kubota, M. (2015). Hope, self-efficacy, optimism, and academic achievement: Distinguishing constructs and levels of specificity in predicting college grade-point average. Learning and Individual Differences, 37 , 210–216.

Ferguson, C. J., Barr, H., Figueroa, G., Foley, K., Gallimore, A., LaQuea, R., Merritt, A., Miller, S., Nguyen-Pham, H., Spanogle, C., Stevens, J., Trigani, B., & Garza, A. (2015). Digital poison? Three studies examining the influence of violent video games on youth. Computers in Human Behavior, 50 , 399–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.021

Ferguson, C. J., & Olson, C. K. (2013). Friends, fun, frustration and fantasy: Child motivations for video game play. Motivation and Emotion, 37 (1), 154–164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9284-7

Gentile, D. A. (2011). The multiple dimensions of video game effects. Child Development Perspectives, 5 (2), 75–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00159.x

Gentile, D. A., & Gentile, J. R. (2021). Learning from Video Games (and Everything Else): The General Learning Model . Cambridge University Press.

Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of violent video game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school performance. Journal of Adolescence, 27 (1), 5–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.002

Gentile, D. A., Swing, E. L., Lim, C. G., & Khoo, A. (2012). Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: Evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1 (1), 62. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026969

Gnambs, T., Stasielowicz, L., Wolter, I., & Appel, M. (2020). Do computer games jeopardize educational outcomes? A prospective study on gaming times and academic achievement. Psychology of Popular Media, 9 (1), 69. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000204

Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. American Psychologist, 69 (1), 66. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034857

Greitemeyer, T. (2018). The spreading impact of playing violent video games on aggression. Computers in Human Behavior, 80 , 216–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.11.022

Griffiths, M., & Lewis, A. (2011). Confronting gender representation: A qualitative study of the experiences and motivations of female casual-gamers. Aloma: Revista de Psicologia, Ciències de l’Educació i de l’Esport , 28 .

Griffiths, M., Kuss, D. J., & Gortari, A. B. O. de. (2013). Videogames as Therapy: A Review of the Medical and Psychological Literature [Chapter]. Handbook of Research on ICTs and Management Systems for Improving Efficiency in Healthcare and Social Care; IGI Global. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-4666-3990-4.ch003

Grooten, J., & Kowert, R. (2015). Going beyond the game: Development of gamer identities within societal discourse and virtual spaces. Loading... , 9 (14), 70–87.  https://journals.sfu.ca/loading/index.php/loading/article/view/151/189

Groves, C. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2015). Negative effects of video game play. Handbook of Digital Games and Entertainment Technologies , 1297–1322. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-4560-52-8_13-1

Haggis-Burridge, M., Passarelli, M., Dagnino, F., Earp, J., Manganello, F., Persico, D., Pozzi, F., Bailey, C., Perrotta, C., & Buijtenweg, T. (2018). A Manifesto for European Video Games . https://doi.org/10.17471/54006

Hale, L., & Guan, S. (2015). Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: A systematic literature review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 21 , 50–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2014.07.007

Hartanto, A., Lua, V. Y., Quek, F. Y., Yong, J. C., & Ng, M. H. (2021). A critical review on the moderating role of contextual factors in the associations between video gaming and well-being. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 4 , 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100135

Hartanto, A., Toh, W. X., & Yang, H. (2018). Context counts: The different implications of weekday and weekend video gaming for academic performance in mathematics, reading, and science. Computers & Education, 120 , 51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2017.12.007

Hawi, N. S., Samaha, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2018). Internet gaming disorder in Lebanon: Relationships with age, sleep habits, and academic achievement. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7 (1), 70–78.

Hellström, C., Nilsson, K. W., Leppert, J., & Åslund, C. (2012). Influences of motives to play and time spent gaming on the negative consequences of adolescent online computer gaming. Computers in Human Behavior, 28 (4), 1379–1387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.023

Indreica, E.-S., Cazan, A.-M., & Truta, C. (2011). Effects of learning styles and time management on academic achievement. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 30 , 1096–1102.

Jackson, L. A., Von Eye, A., Witt, E. A., Zhao, Y., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (2011). A longitudinal study of the effects of Internet use and videogame playing on academic performance and the roles of gender, race and income in these relationships. Computers in Human Behavior, 27 (1), 228–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.08.001

Jordan, P. (2005). Großgliederung Europas nach kulturräumlichen Kriterien. Europa Regional, 13.2005 (4), 162–173.

MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Kahn, A. S., Ratan, R., & Williams, D. (2014). Why we distort in self-report: Predictors of self-report errors in video game play. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 19 , 1010–1023.

Kirman, B., & Lawson, S. (2009). Hardcore Classification: Identifying Play Styles in Social Games Using Network Analysis. In S. Natkin, & J. Dupire (Eds.), Entertainment Computing – ICEC 2009 (pp. 246–251). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-04052-8_28

Ko, C., Yen, J., Chen, C., Chen, S., & Yen, C. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among taiwanese adolescents. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193 (4), 273–277.

Kovess-Masfety, V., Keyes, K., Hamilton, A., Hanson, G., Bitfoi, A., Golitz, D., Koç, C., Kuijpers, R., Lesinskiene, S., & Mihova, Z. (2016). Is time spent playing video games associated with mental health, cognitive and social skills in young children? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 51 (3), 349–357. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-016-1179-6

Krahé, B. (2014). Media violence use as a risk factor for aggressive behaviour in adolescence. European Review of Social Psychology, 25 (1), 71–106. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2014.923177

Kühn, S., Kugler, D. T., Schmalen, K., Weichenberger, M., Witt, C., & Gallinat, J. (2019). Does playing violent video games cause aggression? A longitudinal intervention study. Molecular Psychiatry, 24 (8), 1220–1234. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0031-7

Laconi, S., Tricard, N., & Chabrol, H. (2015). Differences between specific and generalized problematic Internet uses according to gender, age, time spent online and psychopathological symptoms. Computers in Human Behavior, 48 , 236–244.

Lei, H., Chiu, M. M., Wang, D., Wang, C., & Xie, T. (2022). Effects of game-based learning on students’ achievement in science: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Computing Research , 07356331211064543. https://doi.org/10.1177/07356331211064543

Leonhardt, M., & Overå, S. (2021). Are there differences in video gaming and use of social media among boys and girls?—A mixed methods approach. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18 (11), 6085. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18116085

Lopez-Fernandez, O., Williams, A. J., & Kuss, D. J. (2019). Measuring female gaming: Gamer profile, predictors, prevalence, and characteristics from psychological and gender perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology , 10, 1–13.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00898

Mack, D. J., Wiesmann, H., & Ilg, U. J. (2016). Video game players show higher performance but no difference in speed of attention shifts. Acta Psychologica, 169 , 11–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.05.001

Manero, B., Torrente, J., Freire, M., & Fernández-Manjón, B. (2016). An instrument to build a gamer clustering framework according to gaming preferences and habits. Computers in Human Behavior, 62 , 353–363.

McLean, L., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Female gamers: A thematic analysis of their gaming experience. International Journal of Game-Based Learning (IJGBL), 3 (3), 54–71.

Merino-Campos, C., & del Castillo Fernndez, H. (2016). The benefits of active video games for educational and physical activity approaches: A systematic review. Journal of New Approaches in Educational Research (NAER Journal), 5 (2), 115–122. https://doi.org/10.7821/naer.2016.7.164

Mulligan, J., & Patrovsky, B. (2003). Developing Online Games: An Insider’s Guide . New Riders.

Nasrullah, S., & Khan, M. S. (2015). The impact of time management on the students’ academic achievements.  Journal of Literature, Languages and Linguistics, 11 , 66–71.

Nasution, A., Ashraff, M., & Fathil, M. (2015). A study on the relationship between video games and student’s academic performance in IIUM. International Journal of Computer Science and Information Technology Research, 3 (4), 106–111.

Nikkelen, S. W., Valkenburg, P. M., Huizinga, M., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 50 (9), 2228.

Nuyens, F. M., Kuss, D. J., Lopez-Fernandez, O., & Griffiths, M. D. (2019). The empirical analysis of non-problematic video gaming and cognitive skills: A systematic review. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17 (2), 389–414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-9946-0

Paaßen, B., Morgenroth, T., & Stratemeyer, M. (2017). What is a true gamer? The male gamer stereotype and the marginalization of women in video game culture. Sex Roles, 76 (7), 421–435.

Pereg, M., Shahar, N., & Meiran, N. (2013). Task switching training effects are mediated by working-memory management. Intelligence, 41 (5), 467–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.009

Posso, A. (2016). Internet usage and educational outcomes among 15-year old Australian students. International Journal of Communication, 10 , 26.

Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M. A., & Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information processing: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20 (6), 1055–1079.

Prena, K., & Waver, A. J. (2020). Grades on games: Gaming preferences and weekly studying on college GPAs. Game Studies , 20 (4). http://gamestudies.org/2004/articles/prena_weaver . Accessed 10 April 2022

Prescott, A. T., Sargent, J. D., & Hull, J. G. (2018). Metaanalysis of the relationship between violent video game play and physical aggression over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115 (40), 9882–9888. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1611617114

Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2019). Violent video game engagement is not associated with adolescents’ aggressive behaviour: Evidence from a registered report. Royal Society Open Science, 6 (2), 171474. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.171474

Quwaider, M., Alabed, A., & Duwairi, R. (2019). The impact of video games on the players behaviors: A survey. Procedia Computer Science, 151 , 575–582.

Razali, S., Rusiman, M. S., Gan, W. S., & Arbin, N. (2018). The impact of time management on students’ academic achievement. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 995 (1), 012042.

Rivero, T. S., Covre, P., Reyes, M. B., & Bueno, O. F. A. (2013). Effects of chronic video game use on time perception: Differences between sub- and multi-second intervals. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16 (2), 140–144. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0103

Sahin, M., Gumus, Y. Y., & Dincel, S. (2016). Game addiction and academic achievement. Educational Psychology, 36 (9), 1533–1543. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.972342

Schubert, T., Finke, K., Redel, P., Kluckow, S., Müller, H., & Strobach, T. (2015). Video game experience and its influence on visual attention parameters: An investigation using the framework of the Theory of Visual Attention (TVA). Acta Psychologica, 157 , 200–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.03.005

Shaw, A. (2012). Do you identify as a gamer? Gender, race, sexuality, and gamer identity. New Media & Society, 14 (1), 28–44.

Shin, N. (2004). Exploring pathways from television viewing to academic achievement in school age children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165 (4), 367–382. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.165.4.367-382

Silva, L., & Mousavidin, E. (2015). Strategic thinking in virtual worlds: Studying World of Warcraft. Computers in Human Behavior, 46 , 168–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.047

Steinmayr, R., Meißner, A., Weidinger, A., & Wirthwein, L. (2014). Academic Achievement . https://doi.org/10.1093/obo/9780199756810-0108

Steltenpohl, C. N., Reed, J., & Keys, C. B. (2021). Individual behaviors and beyond: Toward a multidimensional view of gamer identity . PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/muhaf

Subrahmanyam, K., & Renukarya, B. (2015). Digital games and learning: Identifying pathways of influence. Educational Psychologist, 50 (4), 335–348.

Taylor, T. L. (2006). Play between worlds: Exploring online game culture . The MIT Press.

The Games Market and Beyond in 2021: The Year in Numbers. (2021). Newzoo .  https://newzoo.com/insights/articles/the-games-market-in-2021-the-year-in-numbers-esports-cloud-gaming/ . Accessed 16 March 2022

Tirgar, H., Balali Meybodi, F., & Hassani, M. (2015). The relationship of playing computer games with aggression and academic achievement: A study on Kerman students. Health and Development Journal , 4 (1), 65–75.  http://jhad.kmu.ac.ir/article_91188.html . Accessed 7 April 2022

Veltri, N., Krasnova, H., Baumann, A., & Kalayamthanam, N. (2014). Gender differences in online gaming: A literature review. Twentieth Americas Conference on Information Systems . Savannah, GA, USA.

Wakil, K., Omer, S., & Omer, B. (2017). Impact of computer games on studentS GPA. European Journal of Education Studies, 3 (8), 263. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.827400

Weaver, J., Kim, P., Metzer, R. L., & Szendrey, J. M. (2013). The impact of video games on student GPA, study habits, and time management skills: What’s the big deal. Issues in Information Systems, 14 (1), 122–128.

West, R., Swing, E. L., Anderson, C. A., & Prot, S. (2020). The contrasting effects of an action video game on visuo-spatial processing and proactive cognitive control. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17 (14), 5160. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17145160

Wolfe, J., Kar, K., Perry, A., Reynolds, C., Gradisar, M., & Short, M. A. (2014). Single night video-game use leads to sleep loss and attention deficits in older adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 37 (7), 1003–1009.

York, T. T., Gibson, C., & Rankin, S. (2015). Defining and measuring academic success. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation, 20 (1), 5.

Zhang, Q., Cao, Y., & Tian, J. (2021). Effects of violent video games on aggressive cognition and aggressive behavior. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2019.0676

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Faculty of Management FAM, University UNION – Nikola Tesla, Belgrade, Serbia

Tijana Savić Tot & Slobodan Adžić

Faculty of Business Economics, University Educons, Sremska Kamenica, Serbia

Faculty of Technical Sciences, University Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia

Maja Aleksić

Faculty of Entrepreneurial Busines, University Union – Nikola Tesla, Belgrade, Serbia

Nebojša Zakić

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Slobodan Adžić .

Ethics declarations

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

All authors certify that they have no affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest or non-financial interest in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.

The authors have no financial or proprietary interests in any material discussed in this article.

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

An ethics statement is not applicable.

All authors contributed to the study conception and design equally.

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available in the Mendeley Data repository, https://doi.org/10.17632/dcp9zb7scx.1

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix: Questionnaire

Do you play video games on electronic devices (PlayStation, XBOX, mobile phone, PC, etc.)?

Have you taken exams in any exam period so far?

What is your current grade point average?

* (open question)

How much time did you study PER DAY on average during the last exam period? How many hours and minutes?

On average, how much time did you play video games PER DAY during the last exam period? How many hours and minutes?

On average, how much time did you spend PER DAY on fun activities, going out, etc. during the last exam period, NOT INCLUDING the time you spent playing games? How many hours and minutes?

What year are you studying?

Partial fulfillment

Master student

PhD student

How old are you?

Are you employed?

What is your gender?

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Savić Tot, T., Adžić, S., Tot, V. et al. The impact of time devoted to video games on student achievement. Educ Inf Technol 28 , 5921–5944 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-022-11418-5

Download citation

Received : 26 August 2022

Accepted : 18 October 2022

Published : 05 November 2022

Issue Date : May 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10639-022-11418-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Video games playing
  • Academic achievement
  • Average grades
  • Higher education
  • Eastern Europe
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research
  • International edition
  • Australia edition
  • Europe edition

The study looked at the correlation between academic scores and the children’s personal interests and activities outside of school, including internet usage.

Positive link between video games and academic performance, study suggests

Students who played online games scored above average in maths, science and reading tests, although study does not prove games were the cause

Children who play online video games tend to do better in academic science, maths and reading tests, according to an analysis of data from over 12,000 high school students in Australia.

The study found that students who played online games almost every day scored 15 points above average in maths and reading tests and 17 points above average in science. However, the study’s methodology cannot prove that playing video games were the cause of the improvement.

Alberto Posso, from the Royal Melbourne Institute of Technology, analysed data from high school students across Australia taking the 2012 Program for International Student Assessment (Pisa) - internationally recognised tests that are administered by the Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). The study looked at the correlation between academic scores and the children’s personal interests and activities outside of school, including internet usage.

Posso, who published the research in the International Journal of Communication , said: “The analysis shows that those students who play online video games obtain higher scores on Pisa tests, all other things being equal.

“When you play online games you’re solving puzzles to move to the next level and that involves using some of the general knowledge and skills in maths, reading and science that you’ve been taught during the day.”

The cause of the association between game playing and academic success is not clear from the research. It is possible that children who are gifted at maths, science and reading are more likely to play online games. Alternatively, it could be that more proficient students work more efficiently, and therefore have more free time, making online gaming a marker of possible academic ability rather than something that actively boosts performance.

Posso also looked at the correlation between social media use and Pisa scores. He concluded that users of sites such as Facebook and Twitter were more likely to score 4% lower on average, and the more frequent the social networking usage, the bigger the difference. 78% of the teenagers said they used social networks every day.

Other studies have found a link between heavy users of social networking and a low attention span , which is also linked to poorer academic performance, but the evidence is less than conclusive.

Peter Etchells, senior lecturer in biological psychology at Bath Spa University , who is not connected with the study, said: “It’s interesting that this study showed a positive correlation between online gaming and academic performance, but we really need better ways of understanding how and why people play video games before we’re able to tease apart what that correlation actually means, if anything.

“A number of researchers have been trying to highlight this issue for a while but we really need more detailed research and nuanced data to answer these sorts of questions more confidently.”

Other studies into the effects of video games on academic performance have shown similar results, either displaying no negative impact or positive effects associated with complex tasks, reasoning and dexterity, as well as reinforcing failure as a learning device. A recent study from Columbia University showed that high video game usage by children aged 6 to 11 was associated with a greatly increased chance of high intellectual functioning and overall school competence.

  • Social media
  • Social networking

Comments (…)

Most viewed.

Logo for Pressbooks@MSL

Chapter 6: 21st-century media and issues

6.5.4 Video games in the world of education (research essay)

Anonymous English 102 Writer

The relationship between education and new technologies, more specifically video games, has long been debated about and judged without a full understanding of the actual potential of the combination of the two. New methods such as using video games as an education tool are almost always overlooked by educators even though they may be missing out on a niche form of education that can keep students far more engaged in the subject they are learning while also encouraging them to continue learning because they are also having fun. It is also often overlooked that playing video games can potentially in some way increase the players’ skills in communication through the usage and improvement of communication required in some games. I share the belief that video games have great potential to be used as a tool for education if more educators would give them a shot and test them out. I also believe that video games have very high potential to improve the communication skills of those who play them. There are also many researchers and educators who share these beliefs and would like to see the use of video games as an education tool more widespread in the future. Many of these researchers and educators that believe in the use of video games as an education tool are of this opinion because they have tested this on students and found results that sway them to see the potential of this form of teaching. Though obviously video games could never become a primary education tool or mainstream form of communication, I think that video games could be utilized well as a form of education and that they could be used by many as a way to improve their communication skills.

Personally, I think video games have a good positive effect on the communication skills of those who play them. I have been playing video games for a large portion of my life and I think the form of communication within some of the more competitive games can have an effect on my communication skills. I think they do this by forcing the player to communicate in the typical form of the game which then have an effect on real world communication skills. In my experience, this increase in communication skill comes from the specific form of communication that happens in competitive video games. This includes games such as one of my personal favorites, Call of Duty Warzone. In order to communicate effectively, the players must communicate through quick and effective bursts of information. In Warzone, my teammates and I must communicate quickly to tell each other important information such as the location of enemies, or where to find valuable resources such as weapons, ammunition and armor. I can do this by either talking to the teammates through a microphone, or using a system called pinging. Pinging is an in game system where you look at an item or location you want to show to your teammates, then hit a button to place a ping there that is displayed to the rest of your team. The most effective way to communicate in Warzone is a combination of both pinging and rapid verbal communication. This way a player can verbally tell their teammates exactly what they are talking about while simultaneously pinging it to show their teammates visually. With the visual indicator on the screen from the ping, and the verbal description from teammates, it should be the most efficient way to tell a teammates something quickly. While some other games also have a pinging system, most force the players to be most reliant on communicating verbally with quick and information packed sentences. This is because while playing these games, the players do not have time to communicate with each other in long detailed sentences. They need to cut down the time it takes to relay information to teammates or often it will be too late for that information to be important anymore. In the time I have played video games I find most of this to be accurate and I believe it has had at least some effect on my communication skills in high stress situations. I also have personal experience with the use of video games education. When I was in elementary school, I frequently used a computer game to help me practice my speed of completing math equations. Even though I was learning the same thing that the teacher was teaching at school, I was much more engaged and interested because a game was more fun that simply doing equations on a sheet of paper. I think that this kind of education can be used much more often to keep students engaged with the subject they are learning. Overall, I have a good connection to this subject and feel confident in the positive relationship between video games and education as well as communication skills.

First, I will explore the research on the topic of the relationship between playing video games and communication skills. A question that I think is important to answer about this topic is how does video game communication effect real world communication skills. Many researchers have studied this relationship to examine video games effects on communication. One such researcher is Kenneth Horowitz in his article “Video Games and English as a Second Language: The Effect of Massive Multiplayer Online Video Games on The Willingness to Communicate and Communicative Anxiety of College Students in Puerto Rico”. In this article Horowitz studies a group of students from Puerto Rico who are attempting to learn English while using video games as one of their forms of learning. As they play these games it works on their English communication skills furthering their knowledge of the language. Horowitz regards new technology and the internet as a whole as a fascinating new potential form of education and more specifically a form of improving the communication skills in those who are learning a new language (Horowitz 379). Because this study revolves around students who are learning English rather than improving on the English skills they already have it is a slightly different situation than the research question asks but still mostly related enough to be relevant. The research was gathered through two questionaries given through the internet to the research participants further relating the connection to the internet and technology to education (Horowitz 391). This means there was a variety of ways for the participants of the study to express the affect the study had on them to diversify the results. While the participants played a wide variety of video game titles and genres, the results and levels of communication between the players did not change but stayed at a similar level relative to each other (Horowitz 398). While the messages players communicate to each other in these different games titles and genres may be different it still requires the same kind of communicative skill to get the message across to teammates effectively. Overall, the relationship between the time of video games played and the skill in communication is a positive correlation (Horowitz 398). When applied to the research question about Video Games correlation to real world communication skills, Horowitz’s research would seem to suggest that Video games do in fact have a positive effect on a person’s real life communication skills in a significant way. Another article that deals with this research question is “Games at Work: Examining a Model of Team Effectiveness in an Interdependent Gaming Task” by Sylvia Luu and Anupama Narayan. This article is another one that deals with how video games and gamification can help people work better as a team in other aspects of life other than video games. The term gamification is used frequently throughout the article, and I think this term is a great term to relate to this essay and the research questions within it as a whole. As stated in the article, gamification could be described as a way of applying skills and aspects of video game behavior into real life situations and circumstances as a way to more efficiently handle them when the need arises (Luu and Narayan 110). The gamification of everyday tasks or especially tasks that have to do with communication and education relates directly to the research question of video games having an effect on real world communication skills. In this article’s experiment a wide range of students volunteered to take part by working together in a video game designed to specifically bring out the teamwork and communication skills of those who played it (Luu and Narayan 114). In contrast to Horowitz’s study, this experiment uses its own game in contrast with Horowitz allowing the participants in the study to play whatever game they chose by themselves. This provides a different point of view and wider range of research data to support this main concept of improvement in communication skill. In the end of the study, it was shown that more communication between the players in the game lead to better performance overall and satisfaction between teammates (Luu and Narayan 116). When the players who participated in the study communicated more it led to a higher rate of success and level of approving between teammates which could be translated to communication in real life scenarios and demonstrates how the communication in video games is not all that different from communication in real life. Yet another article that deals with the ways video games have an effect on a person’s communication skills is “Using video game to enhance English communication skills” by Alex Roach and Yeski Utami. Once again, this article similarly dives into the gamification of education and communication and how video games can incentivize and reward the improvement of communication skills. The article goes over several types of games that can benefit from good communication skills including co-operative games which are directly related to the players working together and communicating as well as they can in order to accomplish their tasks effectively (Roach and Utami 202). Multiple different genres and types of video games have varying degrees of connection to communication and how it can be improved. The research done in this article was based on two different games played by several groups of students in order to diversify and expand the range of results possible to be shown in the data of the experiment (Roach and Utami 203). With multiple different games to play it becomes easier to show and more apparent that the results of one game are not an outlier to the hypothesis. In the results of the experiment, it is believed that the communication between players had a very positive effect on the success in playing the games and that good communication was very important and incentivized for a good performance (Roach and Utami 204). When the students in the experiment communicated better, they performed better in the experiment which shows the strong connection between playing video games and communication skills. Another, more specific article on this topic is “Can game-based learning enhance engineering communication skills?” by Cheyrl Bondar and Renee Clark. Obviously, this article is of a similar topic as the last few but is more specifically relegated to the topic of engineering students using video games in a similar way as the others. This article is based on a study on several hundred engineering students who were asked to write and deliver a presentation on a subject while first using video games as a way to see how the effect the skill in communication among these engineering students (Bondar and Clark 25-26). Though the final presentation these students are composing is not about video games or communication skills, the way video games are implemented into the study still creates an effective situation for the research to show how video games can affect communication skills in these students. The way the video games were used in this study was the students developed their own games specifically designed around different types of communication such as verbal, nonverbal and written communication. These games were meant to test each student in how well they can properly use these different types of communication to complete their assigned final presentations (Bondar and Clark 35). As the students play these several different games with varying objectives, they are also working together closely much like how they will need to for their presentations at the end of the year. In the final results of this study, it was discovered that the positive correlation in communication skills was only found in the level of out loud verbal communication skills (Bondar and Clark 39). This finding shows how playing video games improves the communication each of the players have with and between each other. It seems to show that as the players proceed through the games, they mainly communicate out loud with each other even when playing the games designed around other forms of communication therefor having a much greater effect on their verbal communication skills rather than non-verbal communication skills. Overall, this article and its research show that even when working towards an unrelated goal, video game players who are effectively communicating with each other will see a great improvement in their level of skill in that same kind of verbal communication. My second to last piece of writing on this particular research question is a CNN article titled “Video Games Help Children Improve Literacy, Communication and Mental Well-Being, Survey Finds” by Alaa Elassar. This article and its message are based on a survey administered to several thousand teenagers in a small range of different ages on how they believe playing video games effects their lives. They are asked about how they think playing video games has an effect on multiple different aspects including their skill in reading as well as their writing ability (Elassar). Many people including those in the age range of this survey know that playing video games and reading and writing skills are much more closely related than most would think. These teenagers who participated in the survey also report that the video games they play are a major topic of discussion between them and their friends when they talk (Elassar). While this particular finding may not show direct increase in communication skill related to video games, it does show that playing video games can increase the amount of communicating a person does throughout their day both while playing and when they are not. Many of these teens think playing their video games are a fun and entertaining way for them to build connections and friendships both with people they know in real life and with new people they meet online through the games (Elassar). This shows that playing video games creates and strengthens connections between those who play them together. In a somewhat unrelated note, the participants of the survey also report that playing video games helps reduce their stress and other negative feelings (Elassar). Overall, the playing of video games has a positive effect on most children who play them both in their ability to communicate and on their mental well being. Finally, I have one more online article titled “Video Games Level up Life Skills” by Kathryn Hulick. This article is a simple online piece of writing that conveys the message of how video games can build their valuable life skills such as communication and improvisation or resourcefulness (Hulick). As players play video games, they work on these skills by stimulating their minds in a casual and entertaining way. The article references a study conducted in Scotland about students who played video games during their school week to see if it had any effect on their school performance (Hulick). In comparison to the control group that played no video games during the study did not show as big of an improvement in the traits they were asked to report on, resourcefulness, adaptability and communication skills, as the group of students who did play video games consistently throughout the two months the study was conducted over (Hulick). All of these articles display a very strong connection between communication skills and the amount of video games the players play.

The second part of this essay will be focused around a different connection to video games. The research question for this portion is can video games be used as an effective education tool. The first source I would like to use for this topic is “Video Games Can Develop Graduate Skills in Higher Education Students: A Randomized Trial” by Matthew Barr. This article is based on a trail to show how video games can be used as an education tool. The study is about a group of subjects who were asked to play video games and report how they were affected in a cognitive and social context (Barr 86). The participants play a wide variety of games that have the potential to have educational value to explore many different methods of teaching from a video game. By the end of the research, it was shown that the students involved in the study did show good results for a test administered by the researchers possibly indicating a link between video games and effective communication skills (Barr 90). This shows what my research question aims to ask that video games while not a main source of education can be used as a tool for educating in some instances where it applies and for specific skills. The next source for this research question is titled quite simply “Gaming as a Teaching Tool” written by Brandon Baker. This source is another website article that aims to point out a positive connection between video game playing and engaging education for students. The article discusses many ways video games are being used in various different environments including the military, fitness, and of course schools themselves (Baker). With such a diverse range of games and genres it is possible for video games to be used in hundreds of different contexts and environments as an educational tool in one way or another. Professor Yasmin Kafai certifies that video games have a long extensive history in education and that their modern surge in use is nothing new as Professor Kafai verifies with the course she created titled Video Games and the Virtual World which deals extensively with how video games can be used for educational purposes in many different scenarios (Baker). As Professor Kafai’s course most likely verifies, video games have had a long history of educational purposes even when it may not seem very obvious or apparent. There are many games that have very specific educational values and themes that were made exclusively for classroom education purposes rather than for entertainment purposes such as a game titled Math Blasters (Baker). Although there are some games that are made specifically for educational purposes and not for entertainment purposes, it is still possible and even likely that more entertainment focused video game titles are capable of having educational value whether it is intended or not. Recent events, especially the COVID-19 pandemic, have shown that video games and other technology based methods can be extremely effective and sometimes vital to a learning environments benefit (Baker). When the world of education suddenly became almost entirely reliant on technology to administer its lessons to students it is possible that many educators became at least somewhat aware that video games have a much higher potential of educational use than many would have previously thought. In summation, Baker’s article is very aware and expressive of how the world of education is already very much connected to video games and that they have a bright future in the educational field now that technology has taken on such a crucial role in the field. Another source that deals with this topic is “A New Venue for Video Games: K-12 Classrooms” written by Carolyn Jones. This article begins by describing a teacher who struggled to keep his students engaged and interested in the subject matter until he instituted video games as a way to make his class more engaging and fun for the students. He began to use Minecraft: Education Edition as a part of his history class and witnessed as it drastically increased his student’s engagement with the course material (Jones). When students are given a more entertaining way to learn the same material as a more traditional classroom experience can offer, the entertaining option will usually be much more appealing and effective at keeping the students interested in the material. Other educational games such as Dragon Box focus around other school subjects such as math to provide a wider range of teaching ability across multiple different possible forms of presentation (Jones). Many games have specialty areas of education just like different teachers are more informed on certain subjects. My last source for this question is “The Benefits of Gaming in Education: the Build A World Case” which has no listed author. The article states that gaming has numerous social and psychological benefits to its players and that these benefits have countless positive effects even in the area of education (The Benefits of Gaming). These benefits are often somewhat overlooked but have a very great impact on students and other young people who play video games. As students play video games they are idly and subconsciously improving their critical thinking and technical skills due to the quick snap decisions and puzzle solving that many games have included within them (The Benefits of Gaming). Video games are not often made purposfully with the goal of education in mind but they very often include puzzles and other stimulating challenges that require the player to think critically. Overall These sources all seem to indicate that there is a strong relationship between video games and the field of education.

Works Cited

Horowitz, Kenneth S. “Video Games and English as a Second Language: The Effect of Massive Multiplayer Online Video Games on The Willingness to Communicate and Communicative Anxiety of College Students in Puerto Rico.”  American Journal of Play , vol. 11, no. 3, Jan. 2019, pp. 379–410.  EBSCOhost , search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=EJ1220304&site=eds-live&scope=site.

Luu, Sylvia, and Anupama Narayan. “Games at Work: Examining a Model of Team Effectiveness in an Interdependent Gaming Task.”  Computers in Human Behavior , vol. 77, Dec. 2017, pp. 110–120.  EBSCOhost , doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.025.

Barr, Matthew. “Video Games Can Develop Graduate Skills in Higher Education Students: A Randomized Trial.”  Online Submission , vol. 113, Jan. 2017, pp. 86–97.  EBSCOhost , search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=ED608315&site=eds-live&scope=site.

Roach, Alex, and Yeski Utami. “Using video game to enhance English communication skills.”  Proceedings of ISELT FBS Universitas Negeri Padang  5 (2017): 200-204.

Bodnar, Cheryl A., and Renee M. Clark. “Can game-based learning enhance engineering communication skills?.”  IEEE transactions on professional communication  60.1 (2017): 24-41.

Squire, Kurt. “Video games in education.”  Int. J. Intell. Games & Simulation  2.1 (2003): 49-62.

Baker, Brandon, “Gaming as a Teaching Tool.”  Penn Today , 6 May 2020, penntoday.upenn.edu/news/gaming-teaching-tool.

Jones, Carolyn. “A New Venue for Video Games: K-12 Classrooms  .”  EdSource , EdSource, 7 May 2018, edsource.org/2018/a-new-venue-for-video-games-k-12-classrooms/597100.

“The Benefits of Gaming in Education: the Build A World Case.”  Acer for Education , 27 Jan. 2017, acerforeducation.acer.com/education-trends/gamification/the-benefits-of-gaming-in-education-the-build-a-world-case/.

Elassar, Alaa. “Video Games Help Children Improve Literacy, Communication and Mental Well-Being, Survey Finds.”  CNN , Cable News Network, 5 Sept. 2020, www.cnn.com/2020/09/05/health/video-games-literacy-creativity-children-trnd/index.html.

Hulick, Kathryn. “Video Games Level up Life Skills.”  Science News for Students , 3 Dec. 2019, www.sciencenewsforstudents.org/article/video-games-level-life-skills.

Fishman, Andrew. “Video Games Are Social Spaces: How Video Games Help People Connect.”  Video Games Are Social Spaces: How Video Games Help People Connect | ResponseCenter , www.jcfs.org/response/blog/video-games-are-social-spaces-how-video-games-help-people-connect.

Understanding Literacy in Our Lives by Anonymous English 102 Writer is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here .

Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

Video-Games Do Not Negatively Impact Adolescent Academic Performance in Science, Mathematics or Reading

* E-mail: [email protected]

Affiliation School of Education, Flinders University, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia

Affiliation School of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, United Kingdom

  • Aaron Drummond, 
  • James D. Sauer

PLOS

  • Published: April 3, 2014
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087943
  • Reader Comments

Figure 1

Video-gaming is a common pastime among adolescents, particularly adolescent males in industrialized nations. Despite widespread suggestions that video-gaming negatively affects academic achievement, the evidence is inconclusive. We reanalyzed data from over 192,000 students in 22 countries involved in the 2009 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) to estimate the true effect size of frequency of videogame use on adolescent academic achievement in science, mathematics and reading. Contrary to claims that increased video-gaming can impair academic performance, differences in academic performance were negligible across the relative frequencies of videogame use. Videogame use had little impact on adolescent academic achievement.

Citation: Drummond A, Sauer JD (2014) Video-Games Do Not Negatively Impact Adolescent Academic Performance in Science, Mathematics or Reading. PLoS ONE 9(4): e87943. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087943

Editor: Chris Chambers, Cardiff University, United Kingdom

Received: October 7, 2013; Accepted: February 19, 2014; Published: April 3, 2014

Copyright: © 2014 Drummond, Sauer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Funding: Research was supported by The Sidney Myer Fund and The Myer Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Video-gaming is common among adolescents in industrialized countries, with prevalence rates higher than 75% [1] . Could this common pastime negatively influence adolescents' academic performance? The exciting, fast-paced nature of many videogames could conceivably compromise children's ability to focus on less attention-grabbing tasks (e.g., schoolwork). Consistent with this idea, increased video-gaming has been associated with (a) higher rates of teacher-reports of student attention problems [2] and (b) poorer sleep efficiency [3] . Attentional deficits and poor sleep could both plausibly impair academic performance. Further, increased time spent video-gaming may also reduce home study time and, potentially, academic performance [4] .

Presently, evidence on whether video-gaming negatively affects academic achievement is too weak for causal claims [4] . Although some researchers have reported negative correlations between time spent video-gaming and college students' GPA [5] , and secondary students' school grades [6] , [7] , others have found no relationship between video-gaming and school grades [8] , [9] , [10] . Despite this limited empirical support, the suggestion that video-games may negatively affect academic performance has received widespread media attention (for example, [11] – [14] ). A more comprehensive examination of the effect of video-gaming on academic performance is required.

Specifically, a number of systematic limitations in the extant literature make it difficult to assess the true relationship between academic performance and video-game use. First, most research has used school grades – which contain an inherent subjectivity on behalf of the assessor, and the effects of assessor expectations on student performance are well-documented (see, for example, [15] ) – as outcome measures. A number of widely-cited studies have also relied on students' self-report assessments of their academic performance [7] , [16] – [18] . These two indices of academic achievement are inherently subjective, and thus vulnerable to assessor subjectivity effects. For example, students' self-reports of their average grades may underestimate actual performance in accordance with perceived questionnaire demands [19] , and teachers' preconceptions about students who play videogames may influence their subjective grading of students' performance (for example, [15] ). Using standardized tests of academic performance negates these assessor subjectivity effects. Second, previous research has typically investigated the phenomena across few school sites, which increases the risk of sociocultural factors at particular school sites confounding the results. For example, in particular schools, students who play videogames may also be a peer-group who performs poorly academically, while in other schools the reverse may be true. Alternatively, across schools, the groups of students who play videogames may be more or less homogenous in terms of academic performance. Third, previous research has often used relatively small samples (e.g., 64 participants, [20] ), reducing the reliability of findings.

In contrast to findings based on subjective indices of academic achievement, recent research examining the effects of violent videogames across 333 Hispanic youth revealed little-to-no relationship between a psychometrically valid measure of mathematics performance (the Wide Range Achievement Test-IV , [21] ) and videogame exposure [22] . Thus, although pathological gaming has been consistently associated with poorer academic outcomes [16] , [23] – [25] , recent work indicates that this relationship may not hold for non-pathological game use [24] , [25] . We follow on from this work, and avoid the aforementioned methodological issues, by investigating the relationship between video-gaming and psychometrically valid measures of academic performance in science, mathematics and reading across more than 192,000 students in 7,423 schools within 22 countries.

An additional, potentially important but relatively unexamined consideration is whether the effects of videogames on academic performance vary according to whether the games are played alone (single-player) or in collaboration with others (multiplayer). The social aspects of multiplayer games, together with their inherent reward structures, are intended to increase the games' appeal and the time people spend playing [26] . This increased playing time might result in additional displacement of homework and school related activities, leading to a greater decline in academic performance [4] . Consistent with this idea, participants randomly assigned to play multiplayer (cf. single player) games self-report greater interference in their sleep and academic work [27] .

One concern with random assignment in videogame research is that, in natural settings, people who experience negative effects from gaming can choose not to play, or may have their gaming behavior curtailed. While research with small samples suggests that boys randomly assigned to be given a new videogame console initially perform poorer academically compared to those who have never owned a games console [20] , regular gamers may habituate to the activity, attenuating negative effects on everyday functioning. Further, although providing new videogame consoles to randomly selected children may result in initial declines in academic performance (cf. children who do not possess game consoles), without the appropriate control, it is not clear if this effect reflects properties inherent to gaming behavior or a more general displacement mechanism attributable to the opportunity to engage in a novel activity. If the latter is true, then it might be expected that in an older cohort who have had the opportunity to engage in playing videogames for a greater period of time, the relationship between videogames and academic performance may be weaker or even negligible.

We reanalyzed one of the largest educational datasets ever produced to estimate the true effect size of video-gaming on academic performance among adolescent gamers, and test for differences between single player and multiplayer gamers.

We reanalyzed data from over 192,000 students (aged ∼15 years) across 22 OECD countries assessing the frequency of single- and multiplayer video-gaming (never/hardly ever, once/twice a month, once/twice a week, daily), and including standardized psychometric measures of performance in science, mathematics and reading ability [28] . We present effect sizes in the absence of hypothesis tests as the large number of participants greatly increases the risk of Type-I error.

As video-gaming is most prevalent in Western industrialized nations, we used three criteria for inclusion in the analyses. To ensure that the country was both Western and industrialised, the country had to be an OECD nation, and be classified by the International Monetary Fund as an advanced economy [29] to be included. Second, the country had to have data on the frequency of video-gaming in the PISA dataset [28] . These exclusion criteria left 23 countries. Finally, we excluded South Korea from the analysis due to their non-representatively high prevalence of video-gaming (associated with the rise of e-sports and video-gaming culture). Indeed, the prevalence rate of video-game addiction in South Korea is estimated to be more than double that of any other country [30] . Although data from South Korea were excluded from our primary analyses, we present results including the South Korean data in Figure S1 in the supplementary materials (available online). Within the remaining countries, 192,975 students indicated their frequency of single-player videogame use and 192,741 students indicated their frequency of multiplayer videogame use.

Science, mathematics and reading ability were all assessed on a scale with an international average of approximately 500 and a standard deviation of approximately 100. For reading assessments, students read a section of text and then answered comprehension questions (e.g., interpreting, summarizing, or applying the information contained within the text). For mathematics, students engaged in mathematical calculation and interpretation (e.g., calculating the area of objects or accurately interpreting graphs). For science, students applied scientific thinking (e.g., interpreting the results of scientific experiments, deciding upon the best design for potential experiments, and determining causal factors in particular scenarios) [31] . A complete list of sample questions can be obtained from the OECD [31] .

The 2009 PISA dataset contains five sets of plausible values for each of these constructs. These values represent Rasch model estimates of student performance based upon the differences in test version and, thus, a range of plausible performance for each student. In accordance with OECD [32] recommendations, we analysed all five plausible values separately and present average performance across these analyses. One plausible value for mathematics and science in the analyses of multiplayer gaming failed to converge, as well as one plausible value for mathematics in the analyses of single player videogame use. For these analyses the results were averaged across the remaining four plausible values.

video games affect academic performance essay

Multilevel models allowed the relationship between videogame use and academic performance to vary across countries and schools to obtain the best estimate of the effect of video-gaming on academic achievement [32] . Results are displayed in Figure 1 . As can be seen in the figure, there is no evidence that academic performance in science, mathematics or reading ability, declined as a function of increased gameplay frequency, for single player or multiplayer videogame use. Most differences in student performance across video-gaming frequencies were negligible ( ds <0.2). The largest performance decline associated with increased video-gaming was in reading, with a difference that approached, but did not exceed the cut off for a small effect ( d  = 0.18) between students playing multiplayer games daily and those that never played. All other effects were well below the guidelines for a small effect ( d s<0.2, see Figure 1 ). While the findings relating to mathematical performance support prior research [22] , the negligible and non-existent declines in reading and science associated with increased videogame use contradict previous suggestions that videogames are generally detrimental to academic performance (cf. [22] ). Results including South Korea were qualitatively similar to the results presented here. Note that as ds were calculated using residual variance after accounting for between country and school variances, they potentially overestimate the relationship between video-gaming and academic performance.

thumbnail

  • PPT PowerPoint slide
  • PNG larger image
  • TIFF original image

Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. As MLwiN does not calculate confidence intervals for multi-level models, we estimated confidence intervals as 1.96 times the standard error of the multilevel model slopes, as recommended in the MlwiN Manual [34] .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087943.g001

Table 1 shows the standard deviations for the difference between frequency of videogame play across countries and schools. As can be seen, despite some variance, the results are relatively consistent across countries and school sites.

thumbnail

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087943.t001

We examined the effect of video-gaming on academic performance in an ecologically valid data set, using standardized assessments of academic performance for participants who self-select to engage in video-gaming behavior. Generally, we found little association between video-gaming frequency and academic performance. These data seriously challenge general claims that academic performance is negatively related to the frequency of videogame play (e.g., [4] ).

One explanation for the discrepancy between our results and previous findings may be that PISA's psychometrically valid standardized tests attenuate assessor subjectivity effects inherent in teacher reports and self-reports of school grades (often used as outcome measures in previous research). Research demonstrates that teachers' assessments of student performance are inherently subjective, and are vulnerable to non-veridical influences, including judgments of the student's attitudes and hobbies [15] . Self-reports are similarly vulnerable to non-veridical influences [19] . Another possibility is that those who find that video-games interfere with their schooling may choose not to play or to reduce time spent playing, or have this choice made for them (e.g., by parents). Alternatively, regular gamers may habituate to the activity, attenuating negative effects on academic outcomes.

An advantage of using multilevel modelling analyses is that the relationship between academic performance and video-gaming is allowed to statistically vary across school sites and countries. Within the present data, there was relatively little variance in the relationship between videogame use and academic performance across countries and schools. However, the fact that some variance occurred, particularly at the school level, suggests that increased video-gaming was associated with reductions in academic performance in some schools, but increased performance in others. Thus, with a view to future research, focusing upon any one school is unlikely to provide a good understanding of video-gaming effects on academic achievement. Our results support the need for psychological and educational researchers to examine effects across appropriately large and diverse datasets to ensure their validity.

In sum, across more than 192,000 students in 22 countries, video-gaming behaviour had little effect on psychometrically valid assessments of academic performance in science, mathematics, or reading. The results suggest that the impact of video-gaming on academic performance is too small to be considered problematic.

Supporting Information

Frequency of single-player (top) and multiplayer (bottom) game use and science and mathematics performance including South Korea. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. As MLwiN does not calculate confidence intervals for multi-level models, we estimated confidence intervals as 1.96 times the standard error of the multilevel model slopes, as recommended in the MlwiN Manual [34] .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087943.s001

Acknowledgments

This research used publicly available 2009 PISA data collected by the Organization of Economic Co-operation and Development. We thank R. J. Halsey for support and D. Curtis for statistical advice.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AD JS. Analyzed the data: AD JS. Wrote the paper: AD JS.

  • View Article
  • Google Scholar
  • 6. Anderson CA, Gentile DA, Buckley KE (2007) Violent video game effects on children and adolescents. (Oxford University Press, New York).
  • 11. Dewar G (2013) The effects of video games on school achievement. Available: http://www.parentingscience.com/Effects-of-video-games-on-school.html . Accessed 2013 Jun 09.
  • 12. Gentile DA (n.d.) The effects of video games on children: What parents need to know. Available: http://www.pedsforparents.com/articles/2791.shtml . Accessed 2013 June 09.
  • 13. Isanski B (2010), Game on? Video-game ownership may interfere with young boys' academic functioning. Available: http://www.parentingscience.com/Effects-of-video-games-on-school.html . Accessed 2013 Jun 09.
  • 14. Sabella RA (2010) Negative potential of video games. Available: http://www.education.com/reference/article/negative-potential-video-games/ . Accessed 2013 Jun 09.
  • 15. Rosenthal R, Jacobson L (1968) Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  • 19. Pelham BW, Blanton H (2003) Conducting research in psychology: Measuring the weight of smoke, 2 nd Ed. California: Thomson Wadsworth.
  • 21. Wilkinson G.S, Robertson GJ (2006) Wide range achievement test 4 professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • 28. PISA (2009) Programme for International Student Assessment 2009 Data. Available: http://pisa2009.acer.edu.au/downloads.php . Accessed 2013 April 12.
  • 29. IMF (2013) World economic outlook April 2013: Hopes, realities, risks. Available: http://www.imf.org/external/pubs/ft/weo/2013/01/pdf/text.pdf . Accessed 2013 Aug 19.
  • 30. King DL, Delfabbro PH, Griffiths MD (2013). Video game addiction. In: Miller PM, Editors Principles of Addiction: Comprehensive Addictive Behaviors and Disorders, Volume 1. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 819–827.
  • 31. OECD (2009) Take the test: Sample questions from OECD's PISA assessments. Available http://www.oecdbookshop.org/oecd/display.asp?lang=EN&sf1=identifiers&st1=982009051p1 . Accessed 2013 Nov 11.
  • 32. OECD (2009) PISA data analysis manual SPSS second edition . Available: http://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/pisa-data-analysis-manual-spss-second-edition_9789264056275-en . Accessed 2013 Apr 12.
  • 34. Rasbash J, Steele F, Browne WJ, Goldstein H (2012) A User's guide to MLwiN. Available: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/cmm/software/mlwin/download/manual-print.pdf . Accessed 2013 Apr 12.

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

The influence of playing video games on academic performance among graduates of Karunya University

Profile image of Surendheran K

Digital technology has made a tremendous impact on the life of younger generation, especially the student community. Their socialization process is realized through the means of mobile and Internet engagement. Spending time on technical gadgets is a vital part of their daily life. This attraction towards the digital world limits their academic concentration. This study deals with this problem in order to find out whether the students of Karunya University are engaged in such games that have any impact on their academic performance. Similar studies conducted earlier reveal that the Video games may have a detrimental effect on individual’s GPA and possibly on SAT scores (Anand, 2007). Another study indicates addictive technologies are consistently negatively related to scholastic performance while no such relationship is found for either time spent playing games or video game engagement (Skoric, Linda and Rachel, 2009). To verify this phenomenon, a random sampling survey method is used. The students of various disciplines were randomly selected as respondents of the study. The academic performance of the students is collected from the controller of examinations of Karunya University. The finding reveals the students’ choices in video game genres are significantly correlated with their overall performance indicated by the CGPA (.924). Similarly, the average time spent in such engagement per week is significantly correlated with academic performance which gave out the value of (0.842). The results also reveal that the students’ history of gaming since they were a kid is positively connected with their overall academic performance which is revealed by the correlation value of (0.982). The outcome also shows that the correlation between students’ engagement with video gaming and their academic performance in the first year of their college life is relatively higher than the subsequent years. As a conclusion, video game selection and time spent is significant in Karunya students’ academic performance. The rest of the variables in the study were insignificant. This study can be investigated further to strengthen the findings.

Related Papers

IJHASS Journal

The steps of the gamification approach in education to improve learning engagement are discussed in this paper. There are a few steps on the gamification approach that have been applied by several researchers in this era. The gamification method was used in this study, which contained game elements and game design methods in a non-game context. Six gamification categories for teaching and learning are briefly discussed in this paper: (i) Courses without online support; (ii) MOOC; (iii) Blended/ Flipped Learning; (iv) E-Learning Site; (v) Gamified Platform/ System; and (vi) Mobile Application. The proposed gamification approach in education can also be a guideline for other researchers. Results shows that most of the respondents prefer to get rewards during the learning process follow by level, avatar and points. INTRODUCTION Gamification is the concept of applying game mechanics to engage and motivate students in learning. In other words, games nowadays are a part of students daily life. They spend a lot of time to play games. In order to engage student learning, the gamification method was used, which contained game elements and game design methods in a non-game context. Furthermore, using this method can help to improve learning engagement, suit learners learning style, and provide more adaptive learning. This gamification method has been used by researchers since a few years ago. Past studies showed that there was less engagement among students in building knowledge because often times, the students were treated as ordinary technology users (Tan & Hew, 2016). The solution of accessing this information freely is needed to overcome this situation. Several researchers stated that gamification has the potential for a positive impact on performance, productivity, and user engagement (Simões, Redondo, & Vilas, 2013). By implementing gamification, students will improve themselves in getting good results. The utilisation of the gamification method means that students will learn with academic materials with the use of game elements and cater different styles of learning (Arockiyasamy, Surendheran, & Bullard, 2016; McGrath & Bayerlein, 2013). Engage can be defined when user wanted to learn or to occupy the intentions or efforts of a persons (Keene, 2014).

video games affect academic performance essay

Research in Learning Technology

Matthew Barr

Using a survey of higher education students (N = 2145), correlations between game play habits and the attainment of certain graduate skills or attributes (communication skill, adaptability and resourcefulness) are presented. Correlations between graduate attribute attainment and a range of demographic and educational factors, including age, gender, level of study and year of study, are also calculated. While it is shown that there is no significant relationship between existing game play habits and graduate attribute attainment, several broad observations are made. Students who do not play video games tended to score best, while those students who play games in a variety of modes (online and local cooperative play, team-based and other cooperative play) also scored better on measures of graduate attribute attainment. Assumptions about the development of graduate attributes over time are also challenged by the data presented here, which suggest there is little correlation between attribute attainment and years spent at university. The work suggests that, while video games may be used to develop graduate skills on campus, there is no strong correlation between existing game play habits and the attainment of certain transferable skills.

Garret Ford

Internet Research

Qiaolei Jiang

ABSTRACT Purpose – This paper aims to investigate the interrelationships between Internet connectedness, online gaming, Internet addiction symptoms and academic performance decrement among the Internet-dependent young people in China. Design/methodology/approach – A paper-based survey was conducted among the young clients in one of the earliest and largest Internet addiction clinics in China. A total of 594 in-patients (mean age = 17.76 y) voluntarily participated in this study. Findings –By adopting the concept of Internet connectedness, this study explored the Internet use patterns among the young Internet addicts, for example, Internet café patrons and those who use Internet with more goals or higher degree of Internet adhesiveness had more Internet addiction symptoms. Online gaming was found to play a significant role in the development of Internet addiction. As expected, the level of Internet addiction is significantly linked to academic performance decrement. Consistent with previous studies, males showed higher degree of Internet connectedness and online game usage than females. Noticeably, the moderation effect of online game playing and the mediating effect of Internet addiction were also tested. Research limitations/implications – This research is focused on the Internet-dependent group, thus the generalizability of the results need to be interpreted with caution. Practical implications – This study provides insight for parents, educators, health professionals and policy makers regarding treatment and intervention for Internet addiction among young people in China. Originality/Value – Since very little research has been done focusing on diagnosed Internet-dependent group, this paper scores as a pioneering study of its kind in China.

International Journal of Scientific and Technology Research

Derren N Gaylo

Playing interactive games, both online and offline, might pose risks leading to poor academic achievement. This alarmed parents and school authorities especially that the difference between game engagement and addiction has not been fully understood. The present study determined the influence of interactive games on the mathematical aptitude of secondary school learners. It was conducted at Halapitan National High School, Division of Bukidnon, for the school year 2018-2019, with 208 respondents from Grade 10 and 11 learners. The study utilized descriptive-comparative research design, while employing researcher-made survey questionnaire and National Career Assessment Examination results as main data gathering tools. The appropriate statistical techniques like frequency, percentage, mean, standard deviation, two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) were used to treat the data. Results revealed that the mathematical aptitude of the respondents was at Low Average, showing that the learners showed little knowledge and understanding in the mathematical concepts tested. Moreover, the extent of exposure of the learners in the study is Slightly Exposed to the different levels of interactive games. Further, there is no significant difference in the mathematical aptitude of secondary school learners when grouped according to the levels of interactivity of games they play and their extent of exposure.

Lori Takeuchi

Mediterranean Journal of Social Sciences

Richard Chinomona

Wenielyn Frejoles

John Jones , Nicholas Bowman

This chapter explores the potential correlation between college students’ leisurely video game experi- ence and their narrative composition writing ability in a rst-semester university writing course. This exploratory survey data report moderate correlations between students’ aggregated video game experi- ence (years spent playing) and their ability to articulate tension and turn, and use proper organization in composition assignments, notably a diagnostic essay assigned on the rst day of class, prior to formal instruction. Findings suggest that leisure gameplay might help develop competency with the same cogni- tive and creative skills related to written narrative ability by exposing players – in particular, adolescents – to elements of narrative through the gameplay process, facilitating the learning of these skills in the classroom. In conclusion, the authors suggest areas for future research on this topic.

Ethel Dionaldo

RELATED PAPERS

Uzumymw Kjjdkp

May B . Cermino

meltem huri baturay

vhieve brinas

Carolyn Blume

Steve Sussman

Mahli-Ann Butt

Kamilu Iliyasu

JANAIAH ORIOQUE

Norman Chiliya

Dialogoi! Theory & Praxis in Education 2019, Volume 5, pp. 36-56

Andreas C Oikonomou , Polyxeni Kaimara

World Conference on Educational Multimedia, …

Tzu Yuan Hsu

Emma Westecott

Janry Villegas

China-USA Business Review ISSN 1537-1514 Chinese Business Review ISSN 1537-1506

Carrie Heeter , Rhonda Egidio , Punya Mishra

Jan Van Looy , Thorsten Quandt

Caroline Bayart , David VALLAT

Mark D Griffiths

Mohsen Ebrahimzadeh

Betty Tsakarestou , Georgia-zozeta Miliopoulou , Maria Briana , Konstantinos Ioannidis , Lida Tsene

Communication Management Review

Andrej Malek , Marjan Ninčević

Paul Maharg , Emma Nicol , Kees J.M. Van Haaster , C. N. J. Vey. Mestdagh

Amanda Ochsner

Sandra Annette Rogers

Ana Paulina Suárez Cervantes

Journal of Advances in Humanities and Social Sciences

Sirous Tabrizi

Gamevironments

Robert Houghton

BAMIGBOYE DAMILOLA

Akinlolu G . Ojo

Journal of Media Psychology

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Effects of Video Games Essay

Introduction, school performance, effects on social relationships.

The society has been immensely influenced by the technological changes, which are reshaping some of the activities. The emerging technologies in one way or another have affected every member of society, irrespective of age.

Video Games have had the greatest influence on the lives of children. Children no longer appreciate the outdoor games as was before, as most of their time is spend indoors playing video games.

Although this trend may have a positive impact on the lives of the concerned children as far as their knowledge of computer is concerned, there are various negative impacts.

Various educationists have confirmed that outdoor plays are very important to the development and growth of children.

It helps them develop socially as they meet with their friends and learn to share discussions, get involved in physical activities and develop physically through such games. The paper talks about some of the negative effects of computer games (Finkel, 1995).

It is established through research that computer games do not help children grow academically. In fact, computer games contribute to inactivity of body cells, which might lead to oversight hence causing diseases such as hypertension and diabetes.

Moreover, indoor games contribute to poor academic performance among students because most of the time is spend on useless games that do not offer any academic lessons.

Lastly, computer games lead to the development of antisocial behavior whereby a student is not interested in interacting with his or her peers.

Available literature shows that more children are growing obese owing to the fact that they spend little time exercising and engage frequently with the computer.

Research shows that children who take part in physical exercises are more healthy, intelligent, and active in class. The study conducted by American Heart Association proved that children are contracting heart diseases mainly because of lack of physical activity.

The research indicated that the number of obese children increased by four percent in 1974. Those affected were children aged six to eleven years.

However, the percentage increase could not be compared with the study conducted in 2006 whereby the increase rate was seventeen percent. As from 1971 to 2006, the number of adolescents thought to be obese increased from 6.1 percent to 17.6 percent.

The percentage increase was shocking. Furthermore, the increase was attributed to lack of physical activity and video games (Wiegman, & van Schie, 1998).

Overweight is a costly condition that leads to a number of illnesses among school going children. According to experts at the University of Michigan, obese children have higher risks of contracting diseases such as diabetes, heart diseases, and high blood pressure.

Other illnesses associated with overweight include high cholesterol levels in the body, hyperlipidemia, sleep apnea, respiratory problems, gastrointestinal malfunctions, early adolescence, and finally mental problems.

Studies show that obese children tend to have a low sense of worth and despair.

As AHA records show, obese children are likely to be obese when they attain the adulthood age. This would even cause more problems because they would be exposed to a number of diseases.

A study conducted by Media Literacy Clearinghouse indicated that children had developed a habit towards computer games. Video games had gained relevance among children aged eight and eighteen years to an extent that the rate at which children played computer games rose from an average of 26 minutes in 1999 to 73 minutes in 2009.

Generally, the rate at which children interacted with the computer daily rose from six hours in 1999 to seven hours in 2009. A report compiled in 2004 at the University Hospital of Zurich revealed that a strong link between obesity and video games exist.

It can be concluded that video games do not help children in any way but instead it affects their health. Video games interfere with the children’s eating habits because such children would tend to consume food with high calories.

In this regard, parents need to monitor the behavior of their children as regards to physical fitness.

In the United States, a study conducted to ascertain the effects of computer games on the performance of students proved that PlayStations and other video games such as Xbox Video affects the concentration of children in class.

Furthermore, the study revealed that children who are like using playing video games could not compete favorably with those who prefer physical games. One of the researchers was quoted saying that the performance of students who engage in physical exercises will always improve with time.

Boys are affected more by the new trend since they would rarely improve in class. Their performance would always remain stagnant for a period.

Moreover, boys cannot develop skills that would help them to read and write well in case they are allowed to interact with the computer frequently (Sakurai, 1984).

It should be noted that video games might not necessarily cause poor performance among school going children but the time spent in watching the games or playing them would consume the time that a student would be expected to read, do some homework, and write a good composition.

Definitely, a student would register a poor grade in case he or she does not engage in a serious research. Video games have a tendency of instilling negative reading attitudes to students. This is because a student would find reading a storybook or solving a mathematics problem boring.

Video games are very fast and demand a higher concentration unlike other academic activities that need time for conceptualization. In a joint study conducted in Australia, researchers concluded that video games affect the performance of students in many ways.

Students who spend time playing video games would always perform dismally in class. In fact, the study revealed that computer games do not help children perform well in any of the subjects. This is because students who spend time watching and playing computer games would always score low in each subject.

This research contradicts some of the assumptions that computer games help children to perform well in some subjects. The Australian study revealed that there is no single positive correlation between academic performance and computer games.

Video games are known to influence the cognitive and educational dexterities of children. Moreover, the games can as well as define the children’s social relationships. In a traditional setting, the relationship between the child and the parent or other senior members of society is clearly defined.

The video games have changed meaning that social relations are no longer the same. Studies indicate that video games, especially those that are violent, reshape the behavior of children. Moreover, video games could contain some sort of competition and aggression, which affect the reasoning of children.

In the current society, the rate at which conflicts occur in society has increased. Youths are currently violent because of the new games. The main objective of video games, according to Nintendo and Sega Genesis Center, is violence.

Any game played by children via the computer does not have anything new other than violent content. The companies specializing in selling computer games would convince parents that the games are non-violent but in the real sense, they are not.

A study conducted in 1998 confirmed that many children, over 80%, were familiar to a violent game referred to as Duke Nukem. Unfortunately, only 5% of parents were aware of the game meaning that children are access even those games that are supposed to be accessed by only the adults.

This trend affects the social life of children. In 1999, students at Columbine High School in Littleton, Colorado engaged in a destructive demonstration mainly because of the influence of computer games.

Students shot at their fellow teens and other people because they experienced it through video games. Such kind of behavior is destructive because it could lead to social anomy (Griffith, 1999).

Studies across the world indicate that violent video games increase an individual’s hostility and aggression. Aggressive games would lead to a habit whereby students prefer to play the games rather than engage in physical exercises.

This is even related to overweight. Children who are used to video games tend to be indifferent and unresponsive to the sufferings of other children. They would easily bully their fellow students without mercy.

Finkel, S. (1995). Causal analysis with panel data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Griffith, M. (1999). Violent video games and aggression: A review of the literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior , 4(2), 203-212.

Sakurai, S. (1984). Construction of the Social Desirability Scale for Children. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology , 32(2), 310-314.

Wiegman, O., & van Schie, E. (1998). Video game playing and its relations with aggressive and pro-social behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology , 37(2), 367- 378.

  • Chicago (A-D)
  • Chicago (N-B)

IvyPanda. (2019, December 8). Effects of Video Games. https://ivypanda.com/essays/effects-of-video-games/

"Effects of Video Games." IvyPanda , 8 Dec. 2019, ivypanda.com/essays/effects-of-video-games/.

IvyPanda . (2019) 'Effects of Video Games'. 8 December.

IvyPanda . 2019. "Effects of Video Games." December 8, 2019. https://ivypanda.com/essays/effects-of-video-games/.

1. IvyPanda . "Effects of Video Games." December 8, 2019. https://ivypanda.com/essays/effects-of-video-games/.

Bibliography

IvyPanda . "Effects of Video Games." December 8, 2019. https://ivypanda.com/essays/effects-of-video-games/.

  • Overweight and Obese Children in Miami Dade County
  • The Relationship between playing Violent Video Games and Children’s Aggressive Behavior - What do the Evidences Show?
  • Election (1999) Film Review
  • Child Thinking and Learning New Skills
  • The Ecology of the Family
  • Different Behaviors in Children With Autism
  • Childhood Evolution and History
  • Child and Adolescent Behavior: Becoming Attached

IMAGES

  1. Scope and Delimitation 365

    video games affect academic performance essay

  2. The Effects of Online Gaming to The Academic Performance.docx

    video games affect academic performance essay

  3. The Video Gaming Industry Essay Example

    video games affect academic performance essay

  4. (PDF) Exploring Mobile Game Addiction, Cyberbullying, and its Effects

    video games affect academic performance essay

  5. (DOC) " EFFECTS OF ONLINE GAMES TO THE ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE OF STUDENTS

    video games affect academic performance essay

  6. The academic performance of students Free Essay Example

    video games affect academic performance essay

VIDEO

  1. Gaming Has Gone Too Far

  2. How Video Games Affect Your Brain and Body

  3. History Day State Award Winning documentary

  4. Social Class and Its Impact on Student Performance

  5. The impact of playing games on children and safety issues

  6. Why are Youtube video essays about games so samey? #shorts

COMMENTS

  1. The impact of video games on Students' educational outcomes

    Playing the video games could be very beneficial for the most excellent students. •. Excellent students spend more time on video games, but still they earn high grades. •. The best students winning approach is 'Play hard - Study hard'. •. Video games can act for students as a self-reward scheme. •.

  2. PDF Video Gaming as a Factor that Affects Academic Performance in ...

    parents and students of the potentially harmful effects of playing video games. The article contains 8 tables with two sections each (for male and female participants), and 47 cited entries in the list references. Keywords: video games, adolescence, teenagers, academic performance, school performance, school grades, school attendance

  3. The Impact of Online Games on Student Academic Performance

    Online video gaming has become a popular leisure activity among students, but concerns have been raised about its potential impact on academic performance. While some argue that video games can enhance cognitive skills, others claim that excessive gaming can lead to poor academic performance and even addiction. This research aims to investigate ...

  4. The effects of video game play on academic performance

    Skoric et al (2009) found that while game addiction leads to negative academic performance, moderate engagement in gaming can lead to improved performance in an academic setting. They found a positive correlation between game play Modern Psychological Studies (2011) 17: 37-44.

  5. The Impact of Video Games on Students' Educational Outcomes

    From an educational standpoint, video games can contribute to academic learning by alleviating stress, integrating technical game terminology into classroom content, and serving as a reward system ...

  6. The effects of video game play on academic performance

    Whether playing video games impacts academic performance as determined by GPA is determined by a Gaming Habits Survey which was analyzed using a series of one-way ANOVAs and found that participants who indicated that they did play video games had significantly lower GPAs than participants who did not play videoGames. The purpose of the present research is to determine whether playing video ...

  7. Effect of internet use and electronic game-play on academic performance

    The effects of video game play on academic performance. Mod. Psychol. Stud. 17, 6 ... Terry, M. and Malik, A. Video gaming as a factor that affects academic performance in grade nine.

  8. The impact of time devoted to video games on student achievement

    The aim of this paper is to examine the relationship between the time that higher education students spend playing video games during exam periods and their average grades in one Eastern European country. Moreover, the authors wanted to explore the differences among students with regard to their age, gender, year of study, and employment status in relation to video game-playing habits. Four ...

  9. (PDF) Video Game Engagement and Academic Performance: A Correlation

    To examine the relationship between playing video games and academic achievement, previous studies have employed a variety of research methodologies. Examples of these include surveys, questionnaires, interviews, and analyses of academic records. As a result, more research into how video games affect academic performance is needed.

  10. Effects and Applications of Video Games to Academic Education & Performance

    using video games as education-enhancing tools. Research is. required to see long-term effects of using video game-aided. education in memory, psyche, critical thinking, and stress. management. T ...

  11. Computer Gaming Influence on the Academic Performance Essay (Critical

    The work lists various effects of gaming on academic success and states that multiple factors determine the quality of the final impact (among them there is the length of play, the types of games, the hardware used, and the contents of the games) (Lister, 2016). "Positive link between video games and academic performance, study suggests" by ...

  12. Positive link between video games and academic performance, study

    Children who play online video games tend to do better in academic science, maths and reading tests, according to an analysis of data from over 12,000 high school students in Australia. The study ...

  13. The Effects Of Video Games On Academic Achievement

    Video games have long come under suspicion as having a negative effect on academic achievement. Many studies take the all or none approach, and compare students who play video games to students who do not. The results generally explore whether gaming is associated with poorer academic performance, with limited study as to what aspects, if any ...

  14. 6.5.4 Video games in the world of education (research essay)

    6.5.4 Video games in the world of education (research essay) Anonymous English 102 Writer. April 2021. The relationship between education and new technologies, more specifically video games, has long been debated about and judged without a full understanding of the actual potential of the combination of the two.

  15. Video-Games Do Not Negatively Impact Adolescent Academic Performance in

    Video-gaming is a common pastime among adolescents, particularly adolescent males in industrialized nations. Despite widespread suggestions that video-gaming negatively affects academic achievement, the evidence is inconclusive. We reanalyzed data from over 192,000 students in 22 countries involved in the 2009 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) to estimate the true effect ...

  16. Video Game Engagement and Academic Performance: A ...

    Abstract and Figures. Playing video games is a popular leisure activity among adolescents and youths. People's motivations for playing video games, as well as the potential benefits and drawbacks ...

  17. Does Video Gaming Have Impacts on the Brain: Evidence from a Systematic

    The game genres examined were 3D adventure, first-person shooting (FPS), puzzle, rhythm dance, and strategy. The total training durations were 16-90 h. Results of this systematic review demonstrated that video gaming can be beneficial to the brain. However, the beneficial effects vary among video game types.

  18. (PDF) The influence of playing video games on academic performance

    Another study, A Study of Time Management: The Correlation between Video Game Usage and Academic Performance Markers, from New York conducted by Anand (2007) states that video games may have a detrimental effect on an individual Grade-Point Average (GPA) and possibly on Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) scores.

  19. The Playing Brain. The Impact of Video Games on Cognition and Behavior

    However, the amount of play, regardless of the content, can become harmful when it displaces beneficial activities, affects academic performance or social dimensions [52,121], or supports health problems, such as, for instance, obesity [122,123,124], repetitive strain disorder and video game addiction [76,83]. However, a greater amount of time ...

  20. The effects of computer games on students academic performance

    5 positives impacts that video games have on academic performance 1. They foster cooperation. ... Educational video games not only have a positive effect on students' academic performance but also bring fun to the classroom. And if we show students that learning can be fun, we might very well be able to reignite their intrinsic motivation for ...

  21. (PDF) The influence of playing video games on academic performance

    The result is supported by a study implementing a negative relationship between game addiction and scholastic performance as there are other factors causing poor academic performance such as level ...

  22. Effects of Video Games

    Video games are very fast and demand a higher concentration unlike other academic activities that need time for conceptualization. In a joint study conducted in Australia, researchers concluded that video games affect the performance of students in many ways. Students who spend time playing video games would always perform dismally in class.

  23. Video Games And How They Affect Students' Academic Performance

    Academic performance may get affected by these ailments. The cases might induce discomfort, inflammation, numbness, and paralysis. Also, playing electronic games too much might diminish social connection. Communication skills get harmed by a lack of social engagement. Thus, this might result in social isolation.