Activation Synthesis Theory

practical psychology logo

Sleep discussions generally focus on why we sleep, what happens if we don't get enough sleep, sleep disorders, and even the stages of sleep. But, have you ever wondered why we dream and what any of those dreams we have mean? This is where the Activation Synthesis theory comes in.

The Activation Synthesis theory is the activation of specific brain regions, and its synthesis is what causes dreams. There are bursts of neural activity that stem from the brain cell through the cerebral cortex that, causes the frontal lobe to interpret those signals and give them meaning.

Generally, it is well-known that dreaming comes in the stage of REM. However, are dreams actually significant? Do they have some kind of meaning to them, or is it just something that takes place in the brain and is neurologically based? This has always been something that most people have been fascinated with. Here's all you need to know about the Activation Synthesis theory.

What Is The Activation Synthesis Theory?

The Activation Synthesis theory is generally one of the simplest that most people tend to overthink, or in most cases, they find the term somewhat intimidating. However, the key is understanding what the terms activation and synthesis mean separately. Once that understanding is there, the theory basically speaks for itself.

That is not to say that the model is not complicated. Simply put, activation means that areas of the brain are being used. Then humans need to synthesize or process that information into something that makes sense to us.

The encoding and processing of data are one of sleep's goals. The information from our conscious mind is moved from temporary to permanent memory in the brain during non-REM sleep. Data transfers from the subconscious mind to the long-term memory during REM sleep. During these processes, dreams happen.

Unlike Freud's Wish-Fulfillment or the Information Processing theory, which agrees that dreams have a meaning and a purpose, the Activation Synthesis theory pretty much says the exact opposite. This theory states that dreams are absolutely meaningless.

This theory even further states that dreams are a side effect and do not have a purpose. So, if they are meaningless, then how do they occur? The Activation Synthesis theory theorizes that dreams simply start off as random electrical activation. This random electrical activation originates from the brain stem.

Brain scans have proven that a lot of activity comes from the brain stem when we are in REM while we sleep. For example, the frontal lobe does not stop working in the reticular formation, medulla, etc. The bursts of neural activity that occur when you are asleep are proof of that activity.

Why is the fact that the frontal lobe does not stop working important? This is because the frontal lobe is responsible for interpreting and also creating meanings and those different situations you dream of. Therefore, while we are in REM, the frontal lobe interprets all the bursts of neural activity that come from the brain stem. The frontal lobe is basically just carrying out its function.

Hence why theorists state that dreams are a side effect as the frontal lobe doing its job of interpreting and creating meaning is considered as a side effect of what is happening in your brain stem. Therefore, there is no meaning or any kind of symbolism attached to dreams. [1]

Why Is The Activation Synthesis Theory Well-Liked?

This theory is well-liked because it helps explain why we can have the weirdest dreams that make no sense. Therefore, if you've had dreams that you have had no way of explaining, this theory is the best in getting you to realize that your dream is just that, a dream. There is no deeper meaning or logic to that dream.

There is one other thing that this theory explains: why, every now and then, you can get random noises outside of our dreams making their way into our dream. For example, if someone is knocking on your door in real life and that noise somehow makes its way into your dream. This is how you wake up thinking about how you were dreaming about knocking, and now someone is knocking on your door.

This is simply because the senses don't stop working when you fall asleep. This is easily proven by how you can feel when someone touches your arm while sleeping. The information from outside of your dream is still making its way to your brain, and the frontal lobe will still interpret that sound and whatever else you were dreaming about.  

Therefore, the brain stem activity is the activation part, and the cerebral cortex tries to make sense of what is happening in the synthesis part. [1]

What Is The History Of The Activation Synthesis Theory?

As with almost anything, there is a history to how certain terms came about. The Activation Synthesis theory is no exception. Two individuals in the Department of Psychiatry at Harvard Med School, namely Hobson and McCarley, described the Activation Synthesis model in the 1970s.

This is a model of dreaming, which happens to be an extension of the REMs cycling model. Before this, there are records of Babylonians that go as far back as 5000 years ago, clearly keeping track of dreams and recording them on clay pots. Not only that but also trying to interpret them.

That is still the case in this day and age. There is a list compiled numbering what a dream you had could potentially mean. Some people even use that very same dream list to bet in the lottery. Furthermore, Egyptians actually had a dream God statue they worshiped.

This theory has undergone many revisions over the years since it was created. The revisions happened due to the fact that as the years went by, humans started having increased knowledge of the brain, thanks to technology. The more is learned about the brain, the more theories that are physiologically and neurologically based are further developed. [1]

What Are The Weaknesses Of The Activation Synthesis Theory?

Theories can typically explain things only so far. There will always be some aspect that they cannot quite explain or prove thoroughly. This is the case with the Activation Synthesis Theory, as there are some holes in the theory that dreams have no meaning.

One such weakness is that this theory still does not explain why the frontal lobe in our brain chooses to interpret the activity stemming from the brain stem in the way that they do. I mean, why is a burst of neural activity being interpreted as a mystical creature kind of dream? How did the frontal lobe get to that?

Furthermore, we may have the very same neural activity at night. Still, each frontal lobe may choose to interpret that burst of electrical activity in different ways. This theory cannot explain why the frontal lobe may interpret the neural activity into different imagery for different people even when they have the same burst of neural activity when asleep.

Some other criticisms of this theory are that some people simply don't dream or people that dream but aren't necessarily in REMs sleep. Therefore, some argue that the Activation Synthesis Theory is far too limited as it theorizes that we can only dream in REMs sleep. Also, some feel that it is simply too neurological to truly explain what our dreams are all about.

There is evidence that animals also dream in their sleep. If you're a dog owner, I'm sure you've seen what looks like your dog running in their sleep. That's because they dream of running in their sleep. However, there is no definitive to truly say that it can be fully proven.

Some people feel that because it is mainly based on animal research, it is highly possible that it may not apply to humans as we do not function the exact same way, even though animals typically studied have a similar nervous system.

Furthermore, some FMRI evidence has suggested that the initiation of dreaming occurs in the cerebral cortex and not the brain stem. Ultimately, this dream theory, along with the other, doesn't truly explain why we dream in a way that gives it justice.  [2]

Activation Synthesis In People With Brain Stem Damage

Studies have been conducted on people who have sustained damage to their brain stems. It has been proven that their bursts of neural activity firing from the brain stem are actually not the same as what a person who hasn't sustained damage to their brain stem would be.

However, these people with damaged brain stems are still dreaming. Dreaming does not stop just because the brain stem is damaged. As dreaming still occurs when a brain stem is damaged, and neural activity is still being fired off, the Activation Synthesis Theory is still in effect. [3]

What Happens If The Frontal Lobe Is Damaged?

The brain comprises four lobes, one of them being the frontal lobe. Each lobe has a different function and also controls different behaviors. Unfortunately, the frontal lobe is one of those lobes that is more susceptible to damage as there happen to be many more bony prominences at the front of the skull.

An injury to the frontal lobe will affect the ability to multitask and judgment, but this does not affect dreaming. The science is still up in the air. Still, some evidence suggests that the hippocampus, situated deep inside the temporal lobe, plays a role in dreams.

However, it isn't clear if dreaming stops when the hippocampus is damaged. More research is still to be conducted into whether or not the anatomy linked to the Activation Synthesis Theory stops dreaming when damaged. [5]

What Happens If One Has A REMs Sleep Disorder?

When evaluating the Activation Synthesis Theory, there is one aspect that is particularly interesting as this theory is all about REMs sleep. When focusing on people who do not get the switch of commands, you realize that their brain stem is not damaged and is very active and still sending signals to the brain to alert it to the fact that that individual is in REMs sleep.

However, the muscles just don't comprehend that and, therefore, do not get that switch-off command. This is called a REMs sleep disorder. The subcoeruleus nucleus, a tiny cluster of cells located in the brain stem, regulates REM sleep. Individuals don't really undergo the muscle paralysis linked with REM sleep once those cells are damaged or ill.

This can result in REM sleep behavior disorder, a dangerous condition in which those affected violently play out their dreams. So, what happens when this is the case? Those with this disorder act out what they are dreaming as if they are awake. Take the example mentioned above of a dog acting out running in their sleep. 

Although it is usually certain animals that are more likely to do this. There are humans that have this disorder. There are humans that sleep talk, sleepwalk, sleep eat, etc., but that is not necessarily the disorder. Humans with REMs sleep disorder experience physical, violent outbursts during sleep.

Since the discovery of REMs sleep disorder, research has revealed that three degenerative brain illnesses affect the majority of these patients. The most prominent one happens to be Parkinson's disease, a motor system-affected degenerative condition that occurs in the central nervous system.

Dementia is another type of brain illness caused by REMs disorder. Symptoms include forgetfulness, erratic alertness, visual hallucinations, and difficulty walking. The other well-known condition is multiple system atrophy. This brain illness is known to affect the area of the nervous system that regulates both voluntary and involuntary motions, such as digestion. [4]

People generally have an innate hunger to figure out what dreams mean. Psychologists still aren't 100 percent sure why we dream. Still, there have been several theories that are credible and accepted, one being the Activation Synthesis theory.

Dreams are generally just our brains trying to find meaning in the random signals that the brain stem keeps firing according to the Activation Synthesis Theory. Therefore, trying to put meaning to them is pointless as it all simply has to do with the interpretation of the neural activity.

  • https://www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-activation-synthesis-model-of-dreaming-2794812
  • https://dreams.ucsc.edu/Library/domhoff_2000e.html
  • https://my.clevelandclinic.org/health/body/21598-brainstem
  • https://www.mayoclinic.org/diseases-conditions/rem-sleep-behavior-disorder/symptoms-causes/syc-20352920#:~:text=Rapid%20eye%20movement%20(REM)%20sleep,sometimes%20called%20dream%2Denacting%20behavior .
  • https://www.healthline.com/health/frontal-lobe#:~:text=As%20a%20whole%2C%20the%20frontal,concentrating%20or%20planning%2C%20and%20impulsivity.

Related posts:

  • Why Do We Dream? (6 Theories and Psychological Reasons)
  • Sleep Stages (Light, Deep, REM)
  • Cerebral Cortex (Location, Function, Images)
  • Where is the Primary Visual Cortex Located?
  • Orbitofrontal Cortex

Reference this article:

About The Author

Photo of author

PracticalPie.com is a participant in the Amazon Associates Program. As an Amazon Associate we earn from qualifying purchases.

Follow Us On:

Youtube Facebook Instagram X/Twitter

Psychology Resources

Developmental

Personality

Relationships

Psychologists

Serial Killers

Psychology Tests

Personality Quiz

Memory Test

Depression test

Type A/B Personality Test

© PracticalPsychology. All rights reserved

Privacy Policy | Terms of Use

psychology

Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis

The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis is a theory in psychology that seeks to explain the nature and purpose of dreams. According to this hypothesis, dreams are a result of random brain activity during the rapid eye movement (REM) stage of sleep, which is accompanied by vivid dreaming.

Understanding the Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis

The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis was proposed by J. Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley in 1977 as an alternative explanation to the psychoanalytic theories put forth by Sigmund Freud. While Freud believed that dreams were a reflection of unconscious desires and wishes, the Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis suggests that dreams have no inherent meaning or purpose.

According to this theory, the brain activates various neural circuits during REM sleep, causing random firing of neurons in the brainstem. This random neural activity then stimulates the higher-level brain regions responsible for thinking and perception, such as the cortex. As a result, the brain attempts to make sense of these random signals, leading to the creation of dreams.

Examples of Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis

To illustrate the Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis, consider the following examples:

  • Example 1: A person dreams of flying through the sky and experiencing a sense of weightlessness. According to the hypothesis, this dream may be a result of random activation of brain circuits associated with movement and sensory perception.
  • Example 2: In another dream, someone finds themselves in a chaotic and unfamiliar environment. This dream could be a product of the brain attempting to piece together unrelated fragments of neural activity to create a coherent narrative.

Critiques and Supporting Evidence

While the Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis offers an alternative perspective on dreaming, it is not without its critics. Some researchers argue that dreams may indeed carry symbolic meanings or represent unconscious desires, contradicting the idea that they are purely random manifestations.

However, proponents of the theory highlight various supporting evidence. For instance, brain imaging studies have shown increased activity in the brainstem and decreased activity in the prefrontal cortex during REM sleep, suggesting a connection between neural activation and dream experiences.

The Importance of Understanding Dreams

Regardless of whether one supports the Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis or not, dreams remain an intriguing subject of study in psychology . They provide a window into our subconscious mind, allowing us to explore hidden thoughts, emotions, and memories that may influence our waking lives.

Moreover, dreams can also serve as sources of inspiration, creativity, and problem-solving. Numerous artists, writers, and scientists have attributed their breakthrough ideas to insights gained from their dreams.

The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis proposes that dreams arise from random brain activity during REM sleep. Rather than being purposeful messages or manifestations of our unconscious desires, dreams may simply be the brain’s attempt to make sense of the chaotic neural firing occurring during this stage of sleep. By exploring this hypothesis and studying dreams further, we can gain a deeper understanding of the complexities of the human mind and the role of sleep in psychological processes.

July 26, 2011

The Science Behind Dreaming

New research sheds light on how and why we remember dreams--and what purpose they are likely to serve

By Sander van der Linden

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Getty Images

For centuries people have pondered the meaning of dreams. Early civilizations thought of dreams as a medium between our earthly world and that of the gods. In fact, the Greeks and Romans were convinced that dreams had certain prophetic powers. While there has always been a great interest in the interpretation of human dreams, it wasn’t until the end of the nineteenth century that Sigmund Freud and Carl Jung put forth some of the most widely-known modern theories of dreaming. Freud’s theory centred around the notion of repressed longing -- the idea that dreaming allows us to sort through unresolved, repressed wishes. Carl Jung (who studied under Freud) also believed that dreams had psychological importance, but proposed different theories about their meaning.

Since then, technological advancements have allowed for the development of other theories. One prominent neurobiological theory of dreaming is the “activation-synthesis hypothesis,” which states that dreams don’t actually mean anything: they are merely electrical brain impulses that pull random thoughts and imagery from our memories. Humans, the theory goes, construct dream stories after they wake up, in a natural attempt to make sense of it all. Yet, given the vast documentation of realistic aspects to human dreaming as well as indirect experimental evidence that other mammals such as cats also dream, evolutionary psychologists have theorized that dreaming really does serve a purpose. In particular, the “threat simulation theory” suggests that dreaming should be seen as an ancient biological defence mechanism that provided an evolutionary advantage because of  its capacity to repeatedly simulate potential threatening events – enhancing the neuro-cognitive mechanisms required for efficient threat perception and avoidance.

So, over the years, numerous theories have been put forth in an attempt to illuminate the mystery behind human dreams, but, until recently, strong tangible evidence has remained largely elusive.

On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing . By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.

Yet, new research published in the Journal of Neuroscience provides compelling insights into the mechanisms that underlie dreaming and the strong relationship our dreams have with our memories. Cristina Marzano and her colleagues at the University of Rome have succeeded, for the first time, in explaining how humans remember their dreams. The scientists predicted the likelihood of successful dream recall based on a signature pattern of brain waves. In order to do this, the Italian research team invited 65 students to spend two consecutive nights in their research laboratory.

During the first night, the students were left to sleep, allowing them to get used to the sound-proofed and temperature-controlled rooms. During the second night the researchers measured the student’s brain waves while they slept. Our brain experiences four types of electrical brain waves: “delta,” “theta,” “alpha,” and “beta.” Each represents a different speed of oscillating electrical voltages and together they form the electroencephalography (EEG). The Italian research team used this technology to measure the participant’s brain waves during various sleep-stages. (There are five stages of sleep; most dreaming and our most intense dreams occur during the REM stage.) The students were woken at various times and asked to fill out a diary detailing whether or not they dreamt, how often they dreamt and whether they could remember the content of their dreams.

While previous studies have already indicated that people are more likely to remember their dreams when woken directly after REM sleep, the current study explains why. Those participants who exhibited more low frequency theta waves in the frontal lobes were also more likely to remember their dreams.

This finding is interesting because the increased frontal theta activity the researchers observed looks just like the successful encoding and retrieval of autobiographical memories seen while we are awake. That is, it is the same electrical oscillations in the frontal cortex that make the recollection of episodic memories (e.g., things that happened to you) possible. Thus, these findings suggest that the neurophysiological mechanisms that we employ while dreaming (and recalling dreams) are the same as when we construct and retrieve memories while we are awake.

In another recent study conducted by the same research team, the authors used the latest MRI techniques to investigate the relation between dreaming and the role of deep-brain structures. In their study, the researchers found that vivid, bizarre and emotionally intense dreams (the dreams that people usually remember) are linked to parts of the amygdala and hippocampus. While the amygdala plays a primary role in the processing and memory of emotional reactions, the hippocampus has been implicated in important memory functions, such as the consolidation of information from short-term to long-term memory.

The proposed link between our dreams and emotions is also highlighted in another recent study published by Matthew Walker and colleagues at the Sleep and Neuroimaging Lab at UC Berkeley, who found that a reduction in REM sleep (or less “dreaming”) influences our ability to understand complex emotions in daily life – an essential feature of human social functioning.  Scientists have also recently identified where dreaming is likely to occur in the brain.  A very rare clinical condition known as “Charcot-Wilbrand Syndrome” has been known to cause (among other neurological symptoms) loss of the ability to dream.  However, it was not until a few years ago that a patient reported to have lost her ability to dream while having virtually no other permanent neurological symptoms. The patient suffered a lesion in a part of the brain known as the right inferior lingual gyrus (located in the visual cortex). Thus, we know that dreams are generated in, or transmitted through this particular area of the brain, which is associated with visual processing, emotion and visual memories.

Taken together, these recent findings tell an important story about the underlying mechanism and possible purpose of dreaming.

Dreams seem to help us process emotions by encoding and constructing memories of them. What we see and experience in our dreams might not necessarily be real, but the emotions attached to these experiences certainly are. Our dream stories essentially try to strip the emotion out of a certain experience by creating a memory of it. This way, the emotion itself is no longer active.  This mechanism fulfils an important role because when we don’t process our emotions, especially negative ones, this increases personal worry and anxiety. In fact, severe REM sleep-deprivation is increasingly correlated to the development of mental disorders. In short, dreams help regulate traffic on that fragile bridge which connects our experiences with our emotions and memories.

Are you a scientist who specializes in neuroscience, cognitive science, or psychology? And have you read a recent peer-reviewed paper that you would like to write about? Please send suggestions to Mind Matters editor Gareth Cook, a Pulitzer prize-winning journalist at the Boston Globe. He can be reached at garethideas AT gmail.com or Twitter @garethideas .

AP Psychology Community

AP Psychology Community

AP Psychology Community

AP Psychology Study Resource: Definition Of Activation Synthesis Theory

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

It’s no secret that, as a species, humans are obsessed with the process of dreaming.

We write poems and songs about it. We even have movies about it. A simple google search about dreaming results in thousands of websites where people share their dreams or talk about what their dream interpretations could be.

There are even certain self-proclaimed psychics who claim dreams are a way to see the future.

What’s odd is that there are common dreams, such as teeth falling out or somehow going to work and forgetting to wear clothes. We have all struggled with these silly dreams at one point or another, but how many of us actually understand why?

This is something scientists are continuously trying to figure out. There are a variety of theories about what causes the process of dreaming, although they are all, as of yet, unproven.

One of the most common theories behind dreaming is the Activation Synthesis Theory.

Had Any Good Dreams Lately?

The activation-synthesis theory is a theory based on neurobiological studies into the reasons why we dream.

Since the beginning of time, people have been confused by the process of dreaming. At one point, dreams were alleged to be the chosen method of communication with people from angels or the gods.

Over time, as scientific advancements were made, people began to look at the process of dreaming more skeptically.

Among the people that began to look at dreaming more skeptically were a couple of Harvard neuroscience students named Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley.

Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley were the scientists that first proposed the Activation Synthesis Theory. In 1977, they released a hypothesis that dreaming is caused by the brain trying to make sense of the activity that is still taking place in the brain during sleep.

The brain is the only part of our body that does not rest when we are sleeping. In fact, the brain is always acting at a remarkably high level. The difference when we sleep, according to the hypothesis from Hobson and McCarley, is that the parts of the brain that normally control bodily functions like walking and chewing are now free to take over some of the responsibility of thinking.

How Does Activation Synthesis Work?

The activation synthesis theory is the suggestion that our dreams are caused by these enhanced processes of the brain, which occur when our brain is working entirely on the process of thought. People used to think that sleeping meant we were in a completely passive state. We now know that sleeping is probably the most active period of the day for our brains.

In fact, the brain is almost in overdrive during the time we are sleeping. Our brains work almost like a computer during this time. They are sorting through the activities of the previous day, “filing away” the things we have learned, and making sense of the parts of the day that might have been confusing or overly stressful for us. You might find yourself asking, though, how any of this relates to dreaming.

According to Hobson, when our brains hit the REM cycle of sleeping, our brain begins to sort through the limbic system, which is responsible for emotions, memories, and other such sensations. This is when the process of “making sense” of our thoughts and feelings begins.

Another suggestion from Hobson was that there are five characteristics to dreams that result from this process.

Dreams Are Illogical

Have you ever dreamed you were at dinner with a variety of people that otherwise would not be joining you for dinner? Have you ever dreamed that you went to visit a relative who has passed away? Have you ever dreamed that you were driving a car underwater or flying? Most people have experienced these kinds of ridiculous alternate “realities” while in a dream state.

woman sleeping

  • Search Menu
  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Urban Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Culture
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Lifestyle, Home, and Garden
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Politics
  • Law and Society
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Neuroanaesthesia
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Medical Ethics
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Ethics
  • Business Strategy
  • Business History
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and Government
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic History
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • International Political Economy
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Theory
  • Politics and Law
  • Public Policy
  • Public Administration
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Developmental and Physical Disabilities Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

The Emergence of Dreaming: Mind-Wandering, Embodied Simulation, and the Default Network

  • < Previous chapter
  • Next chapter >

The Emergence of Dreaming: Mind-Wandering, Embodied Simulation, and the Default Network

6 The Activation-Synthesis Theory of Dreaming

  • Published: September 2017
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

This chapter provides the most detailed critical analysis yet developed of the neurophysiological theory of dreaming called activation-synthesis theory, a bottom-up theory that rejects a top-down neurocognitive approach because rigorous studies of dream content allegedly cannot be carried out, especially on the basis of dream reports collected in the sleep lab. This theory suggests that dreams may be “cognitive trash.” The chapter draws on detailed neurophysiological evidence little known outside of neurophysiology, and hardly at all among dream researchers, to show that all of the empirical claims put forth by activation-synthesis theorists in 1977 had been shown to be wrong by the mid-1980s, with further evidence for this refutation appearing in the 2000s due to methodological and technical advances. Similarly, the studies of dream content by activation-synthesis theorists have major flaws and are contradicted by most of the findings presented in earlier chapters.

Signed in as

Institutional accounts.

  • Google Scholar Indexing
  • GoogleCrawler [DO NOT DELETE]

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code

Institutional access

  • Sign in with a library card Sign in with username/password Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Sign in through your institution

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Sign in with a library card

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Our books are available by subscription or purchase to libraries and institutions.

  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Online Learning College

Activation-synthesis theory (Hobson and McCarley 1977)

Activation-synthesis theory (Hobson and McCarley 1977)

In this post

Unlike other research about sleep and dreaming, Hobson and McCarley’s theory focuses specifically on the neurobiological aspects of this, in order to try and explain why and how people dream. This can be quite a difficult concept to understand so make sure you read through each part thoroughly.

Activation synthesis

  Hobson and McCarley argue that dreams occur when the mind tries to make sense of the activity in the brain which is taking place whilst someone sleeps. They also believe that the brain activity that occurs (especially during REM sleep) serves some other purpose, such as restoration and protein synthesis.

The researchers point out that brain activity is present during REM sleep but all muscles are paralysed causing movement inhibition . As well as this, REM sleep means that the brain is not picking up sensory information, which they refer to as sensory blockade . Despite this, they argue that random messages are being sent in the brain and this is the ‘activation’ part of their theory.

These random thoughts are understood to come from neurons in the brain, which are activated and then fired – meaning that an electrical impulse occurs, causing neurochemicals to be released. As we saw in an earlier unit, this means that the neurochemicals will cross a synaptic gap and messages (which are ‘thoughts’ in this case) are sent.

Hobson and McCarley believe that these random thoughts are actually the basis of dreaming as several things come together (or are synthesised) in order to make this happen. The random thoughts, which are triggered by firing neurons, generate internal information. The brain then automatically tries to make sense of this information and this is when a person begins to dream, just as it would try to make sense of information in an environment when the person was awake.

Strengths of the activation-synthesis theory

  • The evidence presented by Hobson and McCarley is compelling; when people sleep in a laboratory and are observed, REM sleep does appear to be a time when dreaming would occur as muscles and senses are not functioning and therefore the brain has to make sense of random information
  • The theory has provided a basis for further investigation of the theory, which is ongoing even today.

Weaknesses of the activation-synthesis theory

  • Other work in this area has been carried out on animals, which cannot be applied to human behaviour
  • Some dreams appear to have specific meaning to the individual, which counteracts the idea that the thoughts that dreams are based on are completely random.

The activation-synthesis theory

Interested in a Psychology GCSE?

We offer the Edexcel GCSE in Psychology through our online campus.

Learn more about our Psychology GCSE courses

Read another one of our posts

Understanding dementia: types, symptoms, and care needs.

Understanding Dementia: Types, Symptoms, and Care Needs

How GCSE Business Prepares You for Real-World Entrepreneurship

How GCSE Business Prepares You for Real-World Entrepreneurship

Preparing for a Career in Adult Social Care: What You Need to Know

Preparing for a Career in Adult Social Care: What You Need to Know

Parent’s Guide to Supporting A-Level Students

Parent’s Guide to Supporting A-Level Students

The Importance of Compassion in Healthcare

The Importance of Compassion in Healthcare

The Role of Palliative Care in End of Life Care

The Role of Palliative Care in End of Life Care

Community Health Initiatives – Promoting Wellness Locally

Community Health Initiatives – Promoting Wellness Locally

Caring for Older People – Strategies for Providing Quality Senior Care

Caring for Older People – Strategies for Providing Quality Senior Care

Save your cart?

Psychology Dictionary

ACTIVATION-SYNTHESIS HYPOTHESIS

speculation which points out dreams as being an output of cortical interpretation of haphazard energizing stemming from the underside of brain tissues by U.S. psychiatrists J. Allan Hobson (193.3- ) and Robert W. McCarleyl

Avatar photo

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Latest Posts

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Meeting the Milestones: A Guide to Piaget's Child Developmental Stages

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Counseling, Therapy, and Psychology: What Is The Difference?

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

The Psychology of Metaphysical Belief Systems

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

4 Key Considerations When Supporting a Loved One Through a Legal Battle for Justice 

How Exercise Can Boost Your Mental Health as You Age

Finding Balance: The Psychological Benefits of Staying Active

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

The Psychology of Winning: Case Studies and Analysis from the World of Sports

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Transitioning to Digital Therapy: Navigating the Pros and Cons

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

From Loss to Liberation: The Psychological Journey Of Seniors Receiving All-On-4 Dental Implants

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

How to Create Family History Interview Questions?

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

The Most Common Addiction in the United States

Road to recovery: tools and resources for mental health treatment success.

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Do Cat Allergy Shots for Humans Work?

Popular psychology terms, medical model, hypermnesia, affirmation, brainwashing, backup reinforcer, affiliative behavior, message-learning approach.

Logo for University of Central Florida Pressbooks

States of Consciousness

Dreams and Dreaming

Learning objectives.

  • Describe and differentiate between theories on why we dream

The meaning of dreams varies across different cultures and periods of time. By the late 19th century, German psychiatrist Sigmund Freud had become convinced that dreams represented an opportunity to gain access to the unconscious. By analyzing dreams, Freud thought people could increase self-awareness and gain valuable insight to help them deal with the problems they faced in their lives. Freud made distinctions between the manifest content and the latent content of dreams.

Manifest content is the actual content, or storyline, of a dream. Latent content , on the other hand, refers to the hidden meaning of a dream. For instance, if a woman dreams about being chased by a snake, Freud might have argued that this represents the woman’s fear of sexual intimacy, with the snake serving as a symbol of a man’s penis.

Freud was not the only theorist to focus on the content of dreams. The 20th century Swiss psychiatrist Carl Jung believed that dreams allowed us to tap into the collective unconscious . The collective unconscious, as described by Jung, is a theoretical repository of information he believed to be shared by everyone. According to Jung, certain symbols in dreams reflected universal archetypes with meanings that are similar for all people regardless of culture or location.

The sleep and dreaming researcher Rosalind Cartwright, however, believes that dreams simply reflect life events that are important to the dreamer. Unlike Freud and Jung, Cartwright’s ideas about dreaming have found empirical support. For example, she and her colleagues published a study in which women going through divorce were asked several times over a five month period to report the degree to which their former spouses were on their minds. These same women were awakened during REM sleep in order to provide a detailed account of their dream content. There was a significant positive correlation between the degree to which women thought about their former spouses during waking hours and the number of times their former spouses appeared as characters in their dreams (Cartwright, Agargun, Kirkby, & Friedman, 2006). Recent research (Horikawa, Tamaki, Miyawaki, & Kamitani, 2013) has uncovered new techniques by which researchers may effectively detect and classify the visual images that occur during dreaming by using fMRI for neural measurement of brain activity patterns, opening the way for additional research in this area.

Woman sleeping.

Recently, neuroscientists have also become interested in understanding why we dream. For example, Hobson (2009) suggests that dreaming may represent a state of protoconsciousness. In other words, dreaming involves constructing a virtual reality in our heads that we might use to help us during wakefulness. Among a variety of neurobiological evidence, John Hobson cites research on lucid dreams as an opportunity to better understand dreaming in general. Lucid dreams are dreams in which certain aspects of wakefulness are maintained during a dream state. In a lucid dream, a person becomes aware of the fact that they are dreaming, and as such, they can control the dream’s content (LaBerge, 1990).

Theories on Dreaming

While the Freudian theory of dreaming may be the most well known, and Cartwright’s suggestions on dreaming the most plausible, there are several other theories about the purpose of dreaming. The threat-simulation theory suggests that dreaming should be seen as an ancient biological defense mechanism. Dreams are thought to provide an evolutionary advantage because of their capacity to repeatedly simulate potential threatening events. This process enhances the neurocognitive mechanisms required for efficient threat perception and avoidance.

The expectation-fulfillment theory  posits that dreaming serves to discharge emotional arousals (however minor) that haven’t been expressed during the day. This practice frees up space in the brain to deal with the emotional arousals of the next day and allows instinctive urges to stay intact. In effect, the expectation is fulfilled (the action is “completed”) in a metaphorical form so that a false memory is not created. This theory explains why dreams are usually forgotten immediately afterwards.

One prominent neurobiological theory of dreaming is the activation-synthesis theory , which states that dreams don’t actually mean anything. They are merely electrical brain impulses that pull random thoughts and imagery from our memories. The theory posits that humans construct dream stories after they wake up, in a natural attempt to make sense of the nonsensical. However, given the vast documentation of the realistic aspects of human dreaming, as well as indirect experimental evidence that other mammals such as cats also dream, evolutionary psychologists have theorized that dreaming does indeed serve a purpose.

The continual-activation theory proposes that dreaming is a result of brain activation and synthesis. Dreaming and REM sleep are simultaneously controlled by different brain mechanisms. The hypothesis states that the function of sleep is to process, encode, and transfer data from short-term memory to long-term memory through a process called consolidation. However, there is not much evidence to back this up. NREM sleep processes the conscious-related memory (declarative memory), and REM sleep processes the unconscious related memory (procedural memory).

The underlying assumption of continual-activation theory is that, during REM sleep, the unconscious part of the brain is busy processing procedural memory. Meanwhile, the level of activation in the conscious part of the brain descends to a very low level as the inputs from the senses are basically disconnected. This triggers the “continual-activation” mechanism to generate a data stream from the memory stores to flow through to the conscious part of the brain.

Link to Learning

Review the purpose and stages of sleep as well as the reasons why we dream in the following CrashCourse video:

You can view the transcript for “To Sleep, Perchance to Dream: Crash Course Psychology #9” here (opens in new window) .

CC licensed content, Original

  • Modification, adaptation, and original content. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY: Attribution

CC licensed content, Shared previously

  • Stages of Sleep. Authored by : OpenStax College. Located at : https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/4-3-stages-of-sleep . License : CC BY: Attribution . License Terms : Download for free at https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/1-introduction
  • The Nature and Meaning of Dreams. Provided by : Boundless. Located at : https://www.boundless.com/psychology/textbooks/boundless-psychology-textbook/states-of-consciousness-6/sleep-and-dreaming-42/the-nature-and-meaning-of-dreams-184-12719/ . License : CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Sleeping woman. Authored by : Craig Adderley. Provided by : Pexels. Located at : https://www.pexels.com/photo/woman-sleeping-1497855/ . License : CC0: No Rights Reserved

All rights reserved content

  • To Sleep, Perchance to Dream – Crash Course Psychology #9. Provided by : CrashCourse. Located at : https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=rMHus-0wFSo . License : Other . License Terms : Standard YouTube License

storyline of events that occur during a dream, per Sigmund Freud’s view of the function of dreams

hidden meaning of a dream, per Sigmund Freud’s view of the function of dreams

common psychological tendencies that have been passed down from one generation to the next

people become aware that they are dreaming and can control the dream’s content

suggests that dreaming should be seen as an ancient biological defense mechanism that provides an evolutionary advantage because of its capacity to repeatedly simulate potential threatening events, thus enhancing the mechanisms required for efficient threat avoidance.

states that dreams don't actually mean anything. Instead, dreams are merely electrical brain impulses that pull random thoughts and imagery from our memories.

proposes that dreaming is a result of brain activation and synthesis; its assumption is that, during REM sleep, the unconscious part of the brain is busy processing procedural memory

General Psychology Copyright © by OpenStax and Lumen Learning is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Experimental Research on Dreaming: State of the Art and Neuropsychoanalytic Perspectives

Perrine m. ruby.

1 INSERM U1028, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Lyon, France

2 CNRS UMR5292, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Brain Dynamics and Cognition Team, Lyon, France

3 University Lyon 1, Lyon, France

Dreaming is still a mystery of human cognition, although it has been studied experimentally for more than a century. Experimental psychology first investigated dream content and frequency. The neuroscientific approach to dreaming arose at the end of the 1950s and soon proposed a physiological substrate of dreaming: rapid eye movement sleep. Fifty years later, this hypothesis was challenged because it could not explain all of the characteristics of dream reports. Therefore, the neurophysiological correlates of dreaming are still unclear, and many questions remain unresolved. Do the representations that constitute the dream emerge randomly from the brain, or do they surface according to certain parameters? Is the organization of the dream’s representations chaotic or is it determined by rules? Does dreaming have a meaning? What is/are the function(s) of dreaming? Psychoanalysis provides hypotheses to address these questions. Until now, these hypotheses have received minimal attention in cognitive neuroscience, but the recent development of neuropsychoanalysis brings new hopes of interaction between the two fields. Considering the psychoanalytical perspective in cognitive neuroscience would provide new directions and leads for dream research and would help to achieve a comprehensive understanding of dreaming. Notably, several subjective issues at the core of the psychoanalytic approach, such as the concept of personal meaning, the concept of unconscious episodic memory and the subject’s history, are not addressed or considered in cognitive neuroscience. This paper argues that the focus on singularity and personal meaning in psychoanalysis is needed to successfully address these issues in cognitive neuroscience and to progress in the understanding of dreaming and the psyche.

The word “dream” is commonly used to express an unattainable ideal or a very deep and strong desire:

I have a dream that my four little children will one day live in a nation where they will not be judged by the color of their skin, but by the content of their character. Martin Luther King

In dream reports, however, one often notices banal situations, strange scenes, or even frightening events. Why is there such a contrast between the popular meaning of the word “dream” and the content of dream reports? Why are some dream scenes so bizarre? Are dreams built from images that arise randomly from the sleeping brain? Or is the emergence and organization of dream images controlled by currently unknown parameters? Does dreaming have a function?

Answering these questions is not easy because dreaming is elusive. We still do not know when it happens during the night, how long it lasts, whether we can recall its entire content, or how to control it. For more than a century, such limited understanding of dreaming has seriously hampered experimental investigations. Nonetheless, scientific research has managed to produce considerable information about the phenomenology and physiology of dreaming and has improved our understanding of this fascinating phenomenon.

Experimental Research on Dreaming

Dreaming and experimental psychology, dream content.

Dreaming was first investigated on an experimental level in the nineteenth century. Calkins ( 1893 ) published the first statistical results about dreaming and argued that some aspects of dream content could be quantified. Later, questionnaires and automatic analysis of the lexical content of dream reports allowed psychologists to show that dream content has some precise phenomenological characteristics. According to psychological studies (Hall and Van de Castle, 1966 ; Schwartz, 1999 ), visual imagery occurs more frequently in dreams than imagery of other senses (audition, olfaction, touch, and taste); the dream drama is mostly lived by the dreamer from a first-person perspective; some elements of real-life events previously experienced by the dreamer often contribute to the scene of the dream; most often, the dream sequence is not within the dreamer’s voluntary control (i.e., the dreamer may be convinced during the dream that the dream’s story is really happening); temporal and spatial incoherencies can occur in the dream story; the dream report is often full of people interacting with each other (e.g., discussions, fights, pursuit, sexuality); and finally, the dream report often contains strong emotions.

Substantial variability of content exists, however, among the same individual’s dreams and among the dreams of different individuals. Further, psychological studies have shown that many internal and external parameters can influence dream content. For example, males report more aggression and violence in their dreams than do females (Nielsen et al., 2003 ; Schredl et al., 2004 ). External stimulation perceived by the dreamer can be incorporated into dreams (Koulack, 1969 ; Saint-Denys, 1867; Hoelscher et al., 1981 ), as illustrated by the famous Dali painting Dream Caused by the Flight of a Bee around a Pomegranate a Second before Awakening . The current concerns of the subject may also be found in the content of his/her dreams (Schwartz, 1999 ; Domhoff and Schneider, 2008 ), and many aspects of the subject’s daily life were found to influence dream content, including news events (Bulkeley and Kahan, 2008 ), musical practice (Uga et al., 2006 ), religious beliefs (Domhoff and Schneider, 2008 ), chronic pain (Raymond et al., 2002 ), mood (Cartwright et al., 1998a ), or a violent living environment (Valli et al., 2005 ). By contrast, congenital or acquired malformations do not seem to significantly influence dream content (Voss et al., 2010 ; Saurat et al., 2011 ).

Based on these results, two opposing hypotheses were formulated: the continuity hypothesis (Schredl and Hofmann, 2003 ) and the discontinuity hypothesis (Rechtschaffen, 1978 ; Kahn et al., 1997 ; Stickgold et al., 2001 ). The former relies on results showing that the themes of an individual’s thoughts during waking life and dreaming are similar; the latter focuses on the fundamentally different structures of thoughts during waking life and dreaming. Voss et al. ( 2010 ) stressed in their recent paper that these hypotheses represent oversimplified approaches to dream analysis and argued that waking and dreaming thoughts were related but structurally independent; in other words, she argued in favor of merging the continuity and discontinuity hypotheses.

Dream report frequency

Dream report frequency (DRF) can vary within subjects and varies substantially among subjects. In a study of 900 German subjects with a large age range from various socioprofessional categories, the mean DRF was approximately 1 dream report per week (Schredl, 2008 ). This result shows that the dream experience is common and familiar to everyone. Psychological studies have demonstrated that many parameters covary with DRF and may thus influence it.

Sleep parameters

First, DRF varies according to the sleep stage preceding awakening (e.g., Dement and Kleitman, 1957b ; Nielsen, 2000 , for a review). More dream reports are obtained after an awakening during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep than after an awakening during non-REM (NREM) sleep. These results inspired the REM sleep hypothesis of dreaming (see the section Dreaming and Neuroscience). Second, DRF increases with the number of awakenings during sleep, according to retrospective self-evaluations of awakenings (Cory and Ormiston, 1975 ; Schredl et al., 2003 ). Such studies showed that the more the subjects tended to awaken during sleep, the higher their DRF. These results support the hypothesis of Koulack and Goodenough ( 1976 ), which proposes that nocturnal awakenings facilitate the encoding of the dream in memory and thus facilitate dream recall upon awakening. However, this hypothesis has not been tested by measuring awakenings with polysomnographic recordings in healthy subjects with various DRFs. Finally, DRF varies according to the method of awakening. Abrupt awakenings lead to more dream reports than gradual awakenings (Shapiro et al., 1963 , 1965 ; Goodenough et al., 1965 ).

Physiological and environmental parameters

Dream report frequency deceases with age (e.g., Schredl, 2008 ) and tends to be slightly higher among females than males (e.g., Schredl, 2008 ; Schredl and Reinhard, 2008 ). Remarkably, Schredl’s ( 2008 ) results revealed that DRF also varied according to the size of the subject’s place of residence.

Psychological parameters

First, increased professional stress or interpersonal stress resulted in an increase in DRF (for a review, see Schredl, 1999 ). Second, an interest in dreams or a positive attitude toward dreams clearly covaries with DRF (Hill et al., 1997 ; Schredl, 1999 ; Schredl et al., 2003 ). The greater an individual’s interest in dreams, the higher his/her DRF. Third, several cognitive abilities have been found to covary with DRF. Contradictory results have been reported for the correlation between DRF and memory abilities (short-term, long-term, visual, verbal, implicit, and explicit; significant positive correlation: Cory and Ormiston, 1975 ; Belicki et al., 1978 ; Butler and Watson, 1985 ; Schredl et al., 1995 ; Solms, 1997 ; no significant correlation: Cohen, 1971 ; Belicki et al., 1978 ; Schredl et al., 1995 , 1997 , 2003 ; Solms, 1997 ) and the correlation between DRF and visual imagery ( significant positive correlation : Hiscock and Cohen, 1973 ; Richardson, 1979 ; Okada et al., 2000 ; no significant correlation : Hill et al., 1997 ; Okada et al., 2000 ). However, several studies have consistently shown that DRF is positively correlated with creativity (Fitch and Armitage, 1989 ; Schredl, 1999 ; Schredl et al., 2003 ) and intelligence scales (multiple-choice vocabulary test, Schonbar, 1959 ; Shipley Intelligence Scale, Connor and Boblitt, 1970 ). Finally, many authors have reported a correlation between DRF and personality traits. Subjects with a high DRF are more likely to have a personality with thinner boundaries (Hartmann described people with thin boundaries as being open, trustworthy, vulnerable, and sensitive; Hartmann, 1989 ; Hartmann et al., 1991 ; Schredl et al., 2003 ), to be more anxious (Schonbar, 1959 ; Tart, 1962 ), to have a higher level of absorption (the absorption scale measures the capacity to become absorptively involved in imaginative and esthetic experiences; Hill et al., 1997 ; Schredl, 1999 ; Schredl et al., 2003 ), to be more open to experience (Hill et al., 1997 ; Schredl et al., 2003 ), and to be less alexithymic (alexithymia is a personality variable that incorporates difficulty identifying and describing feelings, difficulty distinguishing between feelings and the physical sensation of emotional arousal, limited imaginative processes, and an externally oriented cognitive style; De Gennaro et al., 2003 ; Nielsen et al., 2011 ) compared to subjects with a low dream recall frequency. However, those results have not always been reproducible (e.g., Schredl, 2002 for openness to experience; Cory and Ormiston, 1975 ; Hill et al., 1997 for anxiety; Nielsen et al., 1997 for alexithymia) and, according to the recent review by Blagrove and Pace-Schott ( 2010 ), it is difficult to draw conclusions about a possible link between personality traits and DRF.

In conclusion, numerous parameters have been identified that covary with DRF. Schredl stressed in many of his papers that the studied parameters usually explain only a small percentage of the total variance (e.g., Schredl, 2008 ). Thus, the DRF variation profile suggests that the production, encoding and recall of dreams are influenced by numerous parameters that probably interact with each other.

Dreaming and neuroscience

The neuroscientific approach to dreaming arose at the end of the 1950s with the discovery of REM during human sleep by the American physiologist Nathaniel Kleitman and his team (Aserinsky and Kleitman, 1953 ; Dement and Kleitman, 1957a ). During these sleep episodes with saccades, the researchers noticed a decrease in voltage and an increase in frequency in the EEG, accompanied by an increase in cardiac frequency variability and a decrease in body movements. They concluded that these physiological modifications indicate a particular sleep stage, which they called REM sleep. A few years later, the French team led by neurobiologist Michel Jouvet discovered that the lack of movement during REM sleep in cats was due to a general muscular atonia, controlled notably by the locus coeruleus α in the brainstem (Jouvet and Michel, 1959 ; Berger, 1961 later showed that muscular atonia during REM sleep also occurs in humans). Interestingly, the inability to move during REM sleep indicates deep sleep and paradoxically, the fast EEG activity of REM sleep resembles EEG activity in wakefulness. Jouvet concluded that this particular physiological state is associated with a “third state” of the brain (in addition to the brain states associated with wakefulness and NREM sleep) which he called “paradoxical sleep” instead of “REM sleep” (Jouvet et al., 1959 ; Jouvet, 1992 ). Several years later, Fisher et al. ( 1965 ) discovered another physiological characteristic of REM sleep: the penile erection.

During the same period, the American team noticed that a subject awakened during REM sleep very often reported a dream (80% of awakenings in REM sleep vs. 6% of awakenings in NREM sleep are followed by a dream report, according to Dement and Kleitman, 1957b ). Researchers concluded that dreaming occurs during REM sleep. The eye movements of REM sleep would allow the dreamer to scan the imaginary scene of the dream (the scanning hypothesis); the cerebral cortex activation revealed by the rapid EEG would allow intense cognitive activity, creating the complex stories of a dream; and the lack of muscle tone would prevent the dreamer from acting out his dreams. From that time on, researchers investigated REM sleep to obtain answers about dreaming.

In the 1990s, researchers used functional neuroimaging techniques such as positron emission tomography (PET) to investigate brain activity during REM sleep in humans. This new approach enabled researchers to demonstrate that the functional organization of the brain during REM sleep is different from the functional organization of the brain during wakefulness (Maquet et al., 1996 ; Braun et al., 1998 ). In comparison to wakefulness, brain activity during REM sleep is decreased in some brain regions (e.g., in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; Braun et al., 1998 ) and increased in other regions (e.g., in the occipital and temporal cortex, the hippocampus and parahippocampus, the anterior cingulate, the precentral and postcentral gyri, the superior parietal cortex, and the pons; Braun et al., 1998 ; Maquet et al., 2000 ). Looking more generally for brain activity correlating with REM sleep (the vigilance states considered included wakefulness, slow-wave sleep, and REM sleep), Maquet et al. ( 1996 ) found negative correlations in the precuneus, posterior cingulate cortex, temporoparietal junction, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and positive correlations in the amygdala, anterior cingulate, postcentral gyrus, thalamus, and pons (see Schwartz and Maquet, 2002 ; Maquet et al., 2005 ; Nir and Tononi, 2010 for reviews). Based on these results, researchers argued that the particular functional organization of the brain during REM sleep could explain the phenomenological characteristics of dream reports (Hobson and Pace-Schott, 2002 ; Schwartz and Maquet, 2002 ; Maquet et al., 2005 ; Nir and Tononi, 2010 ). They considered that brain activity increases and decreases during REM sleep could be interpreted on the basis of what we know about brain activity during wakefulness. In this context, the increased occipital cortex activity during REM sleep could explain the visual component of dream reports because neuroimaging results during wakefulness showed that visual imagery with the eyes closed activates the occipital cortex (Kosslyn and Thompson, 2003 ). The decreased activity in the temporoparietal junction during REM sleep may explain why dreams are mainly experienced in the egocentric coordinates of the first-person; indeed, during wakefulness, activity in the temporoparietal junction was reported to be greater for allocentric vs. egocentric representation (e.g., Ruby and Decety, 2001 ; Zacks et al., 2003 ) and for third- vs. first-person perspective (e.g., Ruby and Decety, 2003 , 2004 ). The increased activity in the hippocampus during REM sleep could explain why dreams are often composed of known images or characters, as the hippocampus is known to be associated with the encoding and retrieval of lived events during wakefulness (e.g., Piolino et al., 2009 ). The decreased activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex during REM sleep could explain why dream stories lack consistency, why the dreamer’s perception of time is altered, why the dream story is beyond the control of the dreamer and why the dreamer is convinced that the dream story is really happening. Indeed, during wakefulness, the lateral prefrontal cortex is involved in executive function, cognitive control, and working memory (Petrides, 2005 ; Koechlin and Hyafil, 2007 ). The increased activity in the medial prefrontal cortex during REM sleep could explain the attribution of thoughts, beliefs, and emotions to the characters in the dream because, during wakefulness, the medial prefrontal cortex is known to participate in mind reading (Ruby et al., 2007 , 2009 ; Legrand and Ruby, 2009 ). The increased activity in the motor cortex (precentral gyrus) during REM sleep could explain the movements of the characters’ bodies in the dream because, during wakefulness, motor imagery, and the imagination of someone’s action from the third-person perspective involve the precentral gyrus (Decety et al., 1994 ; Ruby and Decety, 2001 ). Finally, the amygdala’s activity during REM sleep could explain why emotions, especially fear, are often mentioned in dream reports; indeed, the amygdala is involved in the processing of emotional stimuli during wakefulness (Adolphs, 2008 ).

In conclusion, results from experimental psychology and neuroscience allow us to better understand the phenomenology of dreaming and the cerebral correlates of some characteristics of dream reports. Still, what do they tell us about the role of dreaming? What are the current hypotheses about dream function(s)?

Hypotheses about dream function(s)

No function.

At the end of the twentieth century, the neurologist Alan Hobson, who was profoundly anti-psychoanalysis, proposed a theory that deprived dreaming of any function. Hobson argued that dreaming is an epiphenomenon of REM sleep: “Because dreams are so difficult to remember, it seems unlikely that attention to their content could afford much in the way of high-priority survival value. Indeed, it might instead be assumed that dreaming is an epiphenomenon of REM sleep whose cognitive content is so ambiguous as to invite misleading or even erroneous interpretation” (Hobson et al., 1998 ).

Psychological individualism

In contrast, other teams, like Michel Jouvet’s, believed that dreaming serves a vital function. In 1979, Jouvet’s team blocked muscular atonia during REM sleep in a cat by damaging the locus coeruleus α in its brainstem. This lesion resulted in the appearance of movements during REM sleep. Movies from the Jouvet lab show sleeping cats performing complex motor actions (with altered control and coordination) resembling those of wakefulness, such as fur licking, growling, chasing prey, mastication, and fighting. From these videos, the authors concluded that the cat was acting out its dream, and they called this non-physiological state “oneiric behavior” (Sastre and Jouvet, 1979 ). These results led Jouvet to propose that dreaming plays a role in reinforcing a species’ typical behavior. Later in his career, Jouvet moved toward a hypothesis focusing on the role of dreaming in the individual dimension. He speculated that dreams (note that, for Jouvet, dreams and paradoxical sleep were equivalent) could be involved in psychological individualism and in the stability of the dreamer’s personality (Jouvet, 1991 , 1992 , 1998 ). According to Jouvet, “the brain is the sole organ of homeotherms that do not undergo cell division. We thus have to explain how certain aspects of psychological heredity (found in homozygote twins raised in different surroundings) may persist for a whole life (psychological individuation). A definitive genetic programming during development (by neurogenesis) is unlikely due to the plasticity of the nervous system. That is why we have to consider the possibility of an iterative genetic programming. The internal mechanisms (synchronous) of paradoxical sleep (SP) are particularly adapted to such programming. This would activate an endogenous system of stimulation that would stimulate and stabilize receptors genetically programmed by DNA in some neuronal circuits. The excitation of these neurons during SP leads to oniric behaviors that could be experimentally revealed – the lists of these behaviors are specific to each individual and indirect data suggest a genetic component of this programming. Amongst the mechanisms allowing the iterative programming of SP, sleep is particularly important. Security – and hence the inhibition of the arousal system – is a sine qua non-condition for genetic programming to take place. In that sense, sleep could very well be the guardian of dreaming” (Jouvet, 1991 ). In other words, Jouvet’s hypothesis is that paradoxical sleep restores neuronal circuitry that was modified during the day to preserve the expression of the genetic program that codes for psychological characteristics. This process would ensure the stability of personality across time.

The threat simulation theory

The Finnish psychologist Antti Revonsuo recently proposed a hypothesis called threat simulation theory, which explains the fearful characteristics of dream content (Revonsuo, 2000 ; Valli and Revonsuo, 2009 ). According to this theory, dreams serve as virtual training places to improve threat avoidance or threat fighting ability. The theory postulates that such nocturnal training makes the dreamer more efficient at resolving threatening situations during wakefulness.

Emotional regulation

Cartwright et al. ( 1998a , b ) defended the idea that dreaming is involved in emotional regulation. Her team showed that, in healthy subjects, the depression level before sleep was significantly correlated with affect in the first REM report. Her team also observed that low scorers on the depression scale displayed a flat distribution of positive and negative affect in dreams, whereas those with a depressed mood before sleep showed a pattern of decreasing negative and increasing positive affect in dreams reported from successive REM periods (Cartwright et al., 1998a ). These results led Cartwright’s team to suggest that dreaming may actively moderate mood overnight in normal subjects. The team strengthened this hypothesis by showing that among subjects who were depressed because of a divorce, those who reported more negative dreams at the beginning of sleep and fewer at the night’s end were more likely to be in remission 1 year later than subjects who had fewer negative dreams at the beginning of sleep and more at the end of the night (Cartwright et al., 1998b ). The researchers concluded that negative dreams early in the night may reflect a within-sleep mood regulation process, whereas those that occur later may indicate a failure in the completion of this process.

Memory consolidation

Finally, a current mainstream hypothesis in cognitive neuroscience credits sleep and dreaming with a role in memory consolidation (for a recent review, see Diekelmann and Born, 2010 ). Numerous studies have shown that brain activity during training is replayed during post-training sleep (e.g., using a serial reaction time task Maquet et al., 2000 , demonstrated replay during REM sleep; using a maze exploration task Peigneux et al., 2004 , demonstrated replay during slow-wave sleep). Decreased performance during the post-training day in sleep-deprived subjects further suggested that the replay of brain activity at night contributes to memory consolidation (e.g., Maquet et al., 2003 ). Only recently, however, have experimental results in humans argued in favor of a role of dreaming per se in memory consolidation. In one study, subjects were trained on a virtual navigation task before taking a nap. Post-nap tests showed that subjects who dreamed about the task performed better than subjects who did not dream (note that only 4 out of 50 subjects dreamed about the task in this study; Wamsley et al., 2010 ). Using a different approach, Nielsen and colleagues provided additional arguments supporting a link between dreams and memory (Nielsen et al., 2004 ; Nielsen and Stenstrom, 2005 ). This team demonstrated that dreams preferably incorporate events that the dreamer lived the day before and events that the dreamer lived 7 days before the dream (U shaped curve). Animal studies have shown that after associative learning, the excitability of hippocampal cells increases (which leads to an increase in neuronal plasticity) and then returns to baseline 7 days after training (Thompson et al., 1996 ). The similarity between the delay of episodic event incorporation into dreams and the delay of post-training cellular plasticity in the hippocampus led the Canadian team to suggest a link between dreaming and episodic memory consolidation.

In summary, the preceding section describes the current state of the art on dreaming, its phenomenology and cerebral correlates and hypotheses about its functions. Some substantial advances have been made, but much remains to be understood.

Unresolved Issues

The link between oneiric behaviors and dream reports.

A piece of evidence in favor of a strong link between REM sleep and dreaming is the oneiric behavior (the appearance of complex motor behaviors when motor inhibition is suppressed during REM sleep) discovered by Sastre and Jouvet ( 1979 ) in cats and reproduced by Sanford et al. ( 2001 ) in rats. Researchers interpreted these results as the animal acting out its dream. However, as animals do not talk, the link between oneiric behavior and dream recall cannot be tested experimentally. This limitation seriously hampers our understanding of dreaming. In humans, complex motor behaviors (e.g., talking, grabbing, and manipulating imaginary objects, walking, and running) can also occur during REM sleep in a pathological context. This syndrome is called REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD). It can be caused by substance withdrawal (e.g., alcohol, Nitrazepam) or intoxication (e.g., caffeine, tricyclic antidepressants) or by various diseases (e.g., Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases, pontine neoplasms). According to physicians experts on this syndrome, some patients report dreams that are consistent with their behaviors in REM sleep (Mahowald and Schenck, 2000 ). According to the literature, however, such matches seem to be loose and not systematic. Only one study has tested whether observers can link dream content to sleep behaviors in RBD (Valli et al., 2011 ). In this study, each video recording of motor manifestations was combined with four dream reports, and seven judges had to match the video clip with the correctly reported dream content. The authors found that reported dream content can be linked to motor behaviors at a level better than chance. However, only 39.5% of video-dream pairs were correctly identified. Note, however, that because the authors obtained only movements and not behavioral episodes for many RBD patients, the link between videos and dream reports was unfairly difficult to make.

It is important to note that motor behavior during sleep can happen outside of REM sleep. Sleepwalking and sleep terrors, which occur during NREM sleep, are usually not considered dream enactments. However, we know that dreams can happen during NREM sleep, and many patients report dreamlike mentation after awakening from sleepwalking or sleep terrors (71%, according to Oudiette et al., 2009 ). In addition, Oudiette et al. ( 2009 ) reported that the dreamlike mentation can correspond with the sleep behavior in NREM sleep. Consequently, the authors concluded that sleepwalking may represent an acting out of corresponding dreamlike mentation.

Recent research suggests that any kind of motor behavior during sleep can be considered an oneiric behavior. One of the challenges for future research is to test the strength of the link between these oneiric behaviors and dream reports in a controlled and systematic way.

Neurophysiological correlates of dreaming

Despite the numerous neuroimaging studies of sleep in humans, the neurophysiological correlates of dreaming remain unclear.

Indeed, dreaming can happen during NREM sleep, and although NREM brain activity differs substantially from REM sleep brain activity (Maquet et al., 2000 ; Buchsbaum et al., 2001 ), some NREM dreams are phenomenologically indistinguishable from REM dreams (Hobson, 1988 ; Cavallero et al., 1992 ; Cicogna et al., 1998 ; Wittmann et al., 2004 ). This phenomenon is difficult to understand given what we currently know about the sleeping brain and about dreaming. One explanation may rely on the possibility that brain activity during sleep is not as stable as we think.

Brain activity during REM sleep in humans is considered to be well understood (Hobson and Pace-Schott, 2002 ; Schwartz and Maquet, 2002 ; Nir and Tononi, 2010 ), but several results question this notion. First, contrary to the common belief that dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity decreases during REM sleep, several studies have reported increased activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during REM sleep (Hong et al., 1995 , 2009 ; Nofzinger et al., 1997 ; Kubota et al., 2011 ). Second, brain activity during REM sleep is heterogeneous. The mean regional cerebral blood flow during 1 min of REM sleep (e.g., as reported in Maquet et al., 1996 ) and the regional cerebral blood flow associated with the rapid eye movements of REM sleep (Hong et al., 2009 ; Miyauchi et al., 2009 ) highlight different brain regions. Finally, few congruencies have been noted in the results of studies investigating brain activity during REM sleep (Hong et al., 1995 , 2009 ; Maquet et al., 1996 , 2000 ; Braun et al., 1997 , 1998 ; Nofzinger et al., 1997 ; Peigneux et al., 2001 ; Wehrle et al., 2005 ; Miyauchi et al., 2009 ; Kubota et al., 2011 ), even between studies using the same technique and the same contrasts (e.g., Braun et al., 1998 ; Maquet et al., 2000 ), or between studies investigating the same REM event (e.g., brain activity associated with rapid eyes movements, as in Peigneux et al., 2001 ; Wehrle et al., 2005 ; Hong et al., 2009 ; Miyauchi et al., 2009 ). Furthermore, few brain regions are consistently reported across the majority of the studies. This inconsistency suggests great intra- and intersubject variability in brain activity during REM sleep in humans. A challenge for future research will be to find out whether the variability in brain activity during REM sleep can be explained by the variability in dream content.

Because dream reports can be collected after awakenings from any sleep stage, one may hypothesize that the brain activity that subserves dreaming (if such brain activity is reproducible across dreams) is quite constant throughout the night and can be observed during all sleep stages. Some results have supported this hypothesis and encouraged further attention in this direction. Buchsbaum et al. ( 2001 ), for example, reported that metabolism in the primary visual areas and certain parts of the lateral temporal cortex does not fluctuate much across REM and slow-wave sleep. Similarly, Nielsen’s team found that dream recall (vs. no dream recall) was associated with decreased alpha (8–12 Hz) power in the EEG preceding awakening, regardless of the sleep stage (Stage 2 or REM sleep; Esposito et al., 2004 ). Interestingly, some authors have suggested that decreased power in the alpha band during wakefulness reflects search and retrieval processes in long-term memory (for a review, see Klimesch, 1999 ).

Processes of selection and organization of dream representations

Nielsen’s team found that episodic events from the 1, 7, and 8 days before a dream were more often incorporated into the dream than were events from 2 or 6 days before the dream (Nielsen et al., 2004 ; results reproduced by Blagrove et al., 2011 ). This result tells us that internal processes control and shape dream content and thus help us to constrain and shape hypotheses about the function and biological basis of dreaming.

At the end of the nineteenth century, Saint-Denys (1867) showed that a sensory stimulus (e.g., the scent of lavender) presented to a sleeping subject without his or her knowledge could induce the incorporation of an event associated with the stimulus (e.g., holidays spent near a lavender field) into the dream, regardless of the delay between the dream and the association stimulus/events (lavender scent/holidays). The author demonstrated that the external world can influence dream content in a direct or indirect way.

Finally, it appears that both external and internal parameters can shape or govern dream content. Nonetheless, few of these parameters are known, and some regularities in the phenomenology of dreams suggest that more influencing parameters remain to be discovered. For example, some individuals experience recurring themes, characters, or places in their dreams. In line with this observation, Michael Schredl’s team showed that the content and style of a person’s life strongly influence dream content (Schredl and Hofmann, 2003 ). However, the rule(s) governing which lived events are incorporated into dreams remain unknown. Do the representations constituting the dream emerge randomly from the brain, or do they surface according to certain parameters? Similarly, is the organization of the dream’s representations chaotic, or is it determined by rules? Does dreaming have a meaning? What is/are the function(s) of dreaming?

Dreaming, Psychoanalysis, and Neuropsychoanalysis

Psychoanalysis, which was developed by the neurologist Sigmund Freud in the beginning of the twentieth century, proposes answers to the questions raised above. Indeed, his theory of the human mind comprises hypotheses about the rules of selection and organization of the representations that constitute dreams.

At the beginning of the twentieth century, Freud presented the concept of the unconscious. He proposed that a part of our mind is made up of thoughts, desires, emotions, and knowledge that we are not aware of, but that nevertheless profoundly influence and guide our behaviors. In his books (e.g., Freud, 1900, 1920 ), Freud proposes that the unconscious mind comes out in slips and dreams. Its expression, however, is coded within dreams (the work of dream), and unconscious thoughts are distorted before they emerge in the conscious mind of the sleeping subject (manifest content of the dream). As a consequence, the dreamer is not disturbed by repressed and unacceptable thoughts (latent content of the dream) and can continue sleeping (this is the reason why Freud considered dreams the guardians of sleep). Hence, according to Freud, decoding dreams’ latent content provides an access to the unconscious mind.

In Freud’s theory of the mind, unconscious thoughts and feelings may cause the patient to experience life difficulties and/or maladjustment, and free unconscious thoughts can help the patient gain insight into his/her situation. As a consequence, Freud developed techniques to decode dreams and provide a way for an analyst to look inside the words and unconscious images of the patient, and to free them through patient insight. One of these techniques is called free association, and is regarded as an essential part of the psychoanalytic therapy process. In order for an analyst to get to the latent content of a dream, he requires the patient to discuss the dream’s manifest content and encourage free association about the dream. Free association is the principle that the patient is to say anything and everything that comes to mind. This includes decensoring his/her own speech so that he/she truly expresses everything. Over time, the therapist or analyst will draw associations between the many trains of uncensored speech the patient shares during each session. This can lead to patient insight into their unconscious thoughts or repressed memories, and the accomplishment of their ultimate goal of “freedom from the oppression of the unconscious” (Trull, 2005 ).

Hence, Freud considered that dreams, as well as slips, have a meaning and can be interpreted, so that one is justified in inferring from them the presence of restrained or repressed intentions (Freud, 1900, 1920 ). Note that, in Freud’s theory of the mind, the words “meaning” and “intention” are closely linked: “Let us agree once more on what we understand by the ‘meaning’ of a psychic process. A psychic process is nothing more than the purpose which it serves and the position which it holds in a psychic sequence. We can also substitute the word ‘purpose’ or ‘intention’ for ‘meaning’ in most of our investigations” (Freud, 1920 ).

In other words, according to Freud, decoding dreams with the free association method provides an access to what makes each of us so special, uncorvering the forces that guide one’s behavior. It gives access to an unknown dimension of ourselves that is fundamental in understanding who we are. It provides access to personal meaning.

This hypothesis, attributing significant importance and meaning to dreams, has rarely been considered by neuroscientists who often consider Freud’s work and theory unscientific.

However, this situation may change as the relationship between psychoanalysis and neuroscience evolves. The starting point was the creation of the International Society for Neuropsychoanalysis in 2000. It was founded by neuropsychologist and psychoanalyst Mark Solms with the intention to promote interactions and collaborations between psychoanalysis and neuroscience. The challenge was serious, as illustrated by neuroscientist Alan Hobson’s aggressiveness in the famous dream debate (Alan Hobson vs. Mark Solms) entitled “Should Freud’s dream theory be abandoned?” held in Tucson, Arizona, in 2006 during the Towards a Science of Consciousness meeting (scientific arguments can be found in Solms, 2000 and Hobson et al., 2000 ). Alan Hobson tried to convince the assembly that Freud was 100% wrong and that Freud’s dream theory was misguided and misleading and should be abandoned. He aimed to demonstrate that Freud’s dream theory is incompatible with what we know about how the brain works. He added that Freud’s dream theory was not scientific because it was not testable or falsifiable. Finally, he presented his model of dreaming, the activation-synthesis hypothesis (Hobson and McCarley, 1977 ; Hobson et al., 2000 ): “The Activation-Synthesis model of dream construction proposed that the phasic signals arising in the pontine brainstem during REM sleep and impinging upon the cortex and limbic forebrain led directly to the visual and motor hallucinations, emotion, and distinctively bizarre cognition that characterize dream mentation. In doing so, these chaotically generated signals arising from the brain stem acted as a physiological Rorschach test, initiating a process of image and narrative synthesis involving associative and language regions of the brain and resulting in the construction of the dream scenarios.” In contrast, Mark Solms demonstrated that what is currently known about the dreaming brain is at least broadly consistent with Freud’s dream theory. He argued that it is generally accepted that brain stem activation is necessary, but not sufficient, to explain the particular characteristics of dream consciousness. What does explain the particular characteristics of dream consciousness, according to Solms, are the following features of brain activity during REM sleep (Braun et al., 1997 ): the activation of core forebrain emotion and instinctual drive mechanisms, i.e., the limbic and paralimbic brain areas (the anterior cingulate, insula, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, and temporal pole), and of the posterior perceptual system (the fusiform gyrus, superior, inferior and middle temporal gyrus, and angular gyrus) and the deactivation of executive dorsolateral frontal control mechanisms (the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex). He further argued that his lesion studies (Solms, 1997 ) are congruent with neuroimaging results because they showed that a total cessation of dreaming results from lesions in the medial part of the frontal lobe and in the temporoparietal junction (whereas no cessation of dreaming was observed for core brainstem lesions or for dorsolateral prefrontal lesions). Finally he emphasized that the activation of motivational mechanisms (such as drives and basic emotions) and of posterior perceptual system associated with deactivation of the executive control (i.e., reality oriented regulatory mechanism) during REM sleep, is broadly consistent with Freud’s dream theory which claims that our instinctual drive states (notably appetitive and libidinal drive system) are relatively disinhibited during sleep. Note that experimental results demonstrating the existence of unconscious representations that guide behavior (e.g., Shevrin and Fritzler, 1968 ; Bunce et al., 1999 ; Arminjon, 2011 , for a review) could also have been cited in support of Freud’s dream theory. This debate was a success for Mark Solms and neuropsychoanalysis. Indeed, at the end of the debate, approximately 100 people voted “no” (i.e., “Freud’s dream theory should not be abandoned”), approximately 50 people voted “yes” and 50 voted “I don’t know”.

Solms’ ( 1997 , 2000 ) approach to dreaming and his experimental results fundamentally challenged our current understanding of dreaming. He proposes that dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms. According to Solms, REM sleep is controlled by cholinergic brain stem mechanisms, whereas dreaming is mediated by forebrain mechanisms that are probably dopaminergic. This implies that dreaming can be activated by a variety of NREM triggers. Several experimental results support this hypothesis.

First, behavioral studies have demonstrated that the link between REM sleep and dream reports is lax. Subjects awakened during NREM sleep can recall dreams at a high rate (Foulkes, 1962 : 74% of awakenings in NREM sleep were followed by dream reports; Cavallero et al., 1992 : 64%; Wittmann et al., 2004 : 60%); dreams can be recalled after a nap consisting only of NREM sleep (Salzarulo, 1971 ; Palagini et al., 2004 ); and some individuals never recall dreams, even when awakened from REM sleep (Pagel, 2003 ). In addition, in healthy subjects with a normal dream recall frequency (around 1 dream recall per week, Schredl, 2008 ), dream recall after an awakening during REM sleep is not systematic: 5–30% of awakenings in REM sleep are not followed by a dream recall, according to the literature (e.g., Dement and Kleitman, 1957a , b ; Foulkes, 1962 ; Hobson, 1988 ). Finally, 5–10% of NREM dreams cannot be distinguished from REM dreams based on their content (Hobson, 1988 ; Cavallero et al., 1992 ; Cicogna et al., 1998 ; Wittmann et al., 2004 ).

Second, as Solms ( 2000 ) argued, the amount of dream recall can be modulated by dopamine agonists (Scharf et al., 1978 ; Nausieda et al., 1982 ) without concomitant modification of the duration and frequency of REM sleep (Hartmann et al., 1980 ). Dream recall can be suppressed by focal brain lesions (at the temporo-parieto-occipital junction and ventromedial prefrontal cortex; Solms, 1997 , 2000 ). These lesions do not have any appreciable effects on REM frequency, duration, or density (Kerr et al., 1978 ; Michel and Sieroff, 1981 ). Finally, some clinical studies suggest that a dream can be triggered by nocturnal seizures in NREM sleep, i.e., by focal brain stimulation. Some cases of recurring nightmares caused by epileptiform activity in the temporal lobe have indeed been reported (Solms, 2000 ).

Conclusion: Collaboration between Neuroscience and Psychoanalysis Would Benefit Dream Research

Considering the issues that remain unresolved (e.g., neurophysiologic variability, parameter(s) influencing the emergence of representations in dreams, the meaning of dreams), a psychoanalytic perspective would certainly benefit dream research by providing new directions/leads and helping to reach a comprehensive understanding of dreaming.

On the one hand, psychological research has demonstrated that dream content is influenced by one’s personal life, especially personal concerns (Schwartz, 1999 ; Schwartz and Maquet, 2002 ; Schredl and Hofmann, 2003 ), and some neuroscientists have hypothesized that dreaming is involved in psychological individualism. Thus, both psychology and neuroscience have provided results and hypotheses that validate the possibility that dreaming has something to do with personal and meaningful issues. On the other hand, Freud argued that the unconscious, which guides behaviors and desires, express itself during dreams. The two disciplines’ (cognitive neuroscience and psychoanalysis) convergence on dreaming thus seems obvious; however, very little collaboration has occurred to date.

Note that some experimental studies in psychology have considered the psychoanalytic perspective. For example, Greenberg et al. ( 1992 ) attempted “a research-based reconsideration of the psychoanalytical theory of dreaming.” They evaluated the presence of problems (defined as an expression of negative feeling or any situation evoking such feeling or requiring some change or adaptation) during dreaming and pre- and post-sleep wakefulness in two subjects. They showed that problems occurred very frequently in the manifest dream content and that these problems were nearly systematically related to the problems noted during pre-sleep wakefulness. In addition, they observed that effective dreams (i.e., dreams that presented some solution to the individuals’ problems) were followed by a waking state in which the impact of the problems was diminished, whereas ineffective dreams were followed by the persistence of the problems. This study thus confirmed that personal concerns influence dream content. In addition it provided new results suggesting that dreaming may have some psychological problem-solving function (this result recalls the neuroscientific findings that sleep has a cognitive problem-solving function associated with brain reorganization; e.g., Wagner et al., 2004 ; Darsaud et al., 2011 ). Greenberg et al.’s ( 1992 ) study managed to quantify personal issues and clearly broadened the cognitive neuroscience perspective on dreaming. To proceed further, approaches integrating psychoanalysis and neuroscience must now be developed. Several subjective issues at the core of the psychoanalytic approach, such as the concept of personal meaning, the concept of unconscious episodic memory and the subject’s history, are not addressed or considered in cognitive neuroscience. This limitation hampers the understanding of psychological and neurophysiological functioning in humans. These issues must be addressed, and the expertise of psychoanalysts in singularity and personal meaning is needed to do so in neuroscience and to further the understanding of dreaming and of the psyche.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

  • Adolphs R. (2008). Fear, faces, and the human amygdala . Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18 , 166–172 10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.006 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arminjon M. (2011). The four postulates of Freudian unconscious neurocognitive convergences . Front. Psychol. 2 :125. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00125 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aserinsky E., Kleitman N. (1953). Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and concomitant phenomena, during sleep . Science 118 , 273–274 10.1126/science.118.3062.273 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Belicki K., Hunt H., Kelly P. (1978). The function of dream and dreamer variables in the question of dream recall . Sleep Res. 7 , 167 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berger R. J. (1961). Tonus of extrinsic laryngeal muscles during sleep and dreaming . Science 134 , 840. 10.1126/science.134.3482.840 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blagrove M., Henley-Einion J., Barnett A., Edwards D., Heidi Seage C. (2011). A replication of the 5-7 day dream-lag effect with comparison of dreams to future events as control for baseline matching . Conscious. Cogn. 20 , 384–391 10.1016/j.concog.2010.07.006 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blagrove M., Pace-Schott E. F. (2010). Trait and neurobiological correlates of individual differences in dream recall and dream content . Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 92 , 155–180 10.1016/S0074-7742(10)92008-4 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braun A. R., Balkin T. J., Wesensten N. J., Gwadry F., Carson R. E., Varga M., Baldwin P., Belenky G., Herscovitch P. (1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep . Science 279 , 91–95 10.1126/science.279.5347.91 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Braun A. R., Balkin T. J., Wesenten N. J., Carson R. E., Varga M., Baldwin P., Selbie S., Belenky G., Herscovitch P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H215O PET study . Brain 120 ( Pt 7 ), 1173–1197 10.1093/brain/120.5.761 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buchsbaum M. S., Hazlett E. A., Wu J., Bunney W. E., Jr. (2001). Positron emission tomography with deoxyglucose-F18 imaging of sleep . Neuropsychopharmacology 25 , S50–S56 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00339-6 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bulkeley K., Kahan T. L. (2008). The impact of September 11 on dreaming . Conscious. Cogn. 17 , 1248–1256 10.1016/j.concog.2008.07.001 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bunce S. C., Bernat E., Wong P. S., Shevrin H. (1999). Further evidence for unconscious learning: preliminary support for the conditioning of facial EMG to subliminal stimuli . J. Psychiatr. Res. 33 , 341–347 10.1016/S0022-3956(99)00003-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Butler S. F., Watson R. (1985). Individual differences in memory for dreams: the role of cognitive skills . Percept. Mot. Skills 61 , 823–828 10.2466/pms.1985.61.3.823 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Calkins M. W. (1893). Statistics of dreams . Am. J. Psychol. 5 , 311–343 10.2307/1411924 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cartwright R., Luten A., Young M., Mercer P., Bears M. (1998a). Role of REM sleep and dream affect in overnight mood regulation: a study of normal volunteers . Psychiatry Res. 81 , 1–8 10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00089-4 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cartwright R., Young M. A., Mercer P., Bears M. (1998b). Role of REM sleep and dream variables in the prediction of remission from depression . Psychiatry Res. 80 , 249–255 10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00071-7 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cavallero C., Cicogna P., Natale V., Occhionero M., Zito A. (1992). Slow wave sleep dreaming . Sleep 15 , 562–566 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cicogna P. C., Natale V., Occhionero M., Bosinelli M. (1998). A comparison of mental activity during sleep onset and morning awakening . Sleep 21 , 462–470 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cohen D. B. (1971). Dream recall and short-term memory . Percept. Mot. Skills 33 , 867–871 10.2466/pms.1971.33.1.101 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Connor G. N., Boblitt W. M. E. (1970). Reported frequency of dream recall as a function of intelligence and various personality test factors . J. Clin. Psychol. 26 , 438–439 10.1002/1097-4679(197010)26:4<438::AID-JCLP2270260410>3.0.CO;2-X [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cory T. L., Ormiston D. W. (1975). Predicting the frequency of dream recall . J. Abnorm. Psychol. 84 , 261–266 10.1037/h0076653 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Darsaud A., Wagner U., Balteau E., Desseilles M., Sterpenich V., Vandewalle G., Albouy G., Dang-Vu T., Collette F., Boly M., Schabus M., Degueldre C., Luxen A., Maquet P. (2011). Neural precursors of delayed insight . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23 , 1900–1910 10.1162/jocn.2010.21448 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • De Gennaro L., Ferrara M., Cristiani R., Curcio G., Martiradonna V., Bertini M. (2003). Alexithymia and dream recall upon spontaneous morning awakening . Psychosom. Med. 65 , 301–306 10.1097/01.PSY.0000058373.50240.71 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Decety J., Perani D., Jeannerod M., Bettinardi V., Tadary B., Woods R., Mazziotta J. C., Fazio F. (1994). Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography . Nature 371 , 600–602 10.1038/371600a0 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dement W., Kleitman N. (1957a). Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming . Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 9 , 673–690 10.1016/0013-4694(57)90088-3 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dement W., Kleitman N. (1957b). The relation of eye movements during sleep to dream activity: an objective method for the study of dreaming . J. Exp. Psychol. 53 , 339–346 10.1037/h0048189 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diekelmann S., Born J. (2010). The memory function of sleep . Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11 , 114–126 10.1038/nrn2762-c2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Domhoff G. W., Schneider A. (2008). Studying dream content using the archive and search engine on DreamBank.net . Conscious. Cogn. 17 , 1238–1247 10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.010 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Esposito M. J., Nielsen T. A., Paquette T. (2004). Reduced alpha power associated with the recall of mentation from Stage 2 and Stage REM sleep . Psychophysiology 41 , 288–297 10.1111/j.1469-8986.00143.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fisher C., Gorss J., Zuch J. (1965). Cycle of penile erection synchronous with dreaming (Rem) sleep. Preliminary report . Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 12 , 29–45 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fitch T., Armitage R. (1989). Variations in cognitive style among high and low frequency dream recallers . Pers. Individ. dif. 10 , 869–875 10.1016/0191-8869(89)90022-6 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Foulkes W. D. (1962). Dream reports from different stages of sleep . J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 65 , 14–25 10.1037/h0040431 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Freud S. (1967). L’interprétation des rẽves (I. Meyerson, Trans.) . Paris: PUF. (Original work published 1900) [ Google Scholar ]
  • Freud S. (1920). A General Introduction to Psychoanalysis . New York: Boni and Liveright publishers [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodenough D. R., Lewis H. B., Shapiro A., Jaret L., Sleser I. (1965). Dream reporting following abrupt and gradual awakenings from different types of sleep . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56 , 170–179 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenberg R., Katz H., Schwartz W., Pearlman C. (1992). A research-based reconsideration of the psychoanalytic theory of dreaming . J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 40 , 531–550 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hall C. S., Van de Castle R. L. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams . New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hartmann E. (1989). Boundaries of dreams, boundaries of dreamers: thin and thick boundaries as a new personality measure . Psychiatr. J. Univ. Ott. 14 , 557–560 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hartmann E., Elkin R., Garg M. (1991). Personality and dreaming: the dreams of people with very thick or very thin boundaries . Dreaming 1 , 311–324 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hartmann E., Russ D., Oldfield M., Falke R., Skoff B. (1980). Dream content: effects of l-DOPA . Sleep Res. 9 , 153 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hill C. E., Diemer R. A., Heaton K. J. (1997). Dream interpretation sessions: who volunteers, who benefits, and what volunteer clients view as most and least helpful . J. Couns. Psychol. 44 , 53–62 10.1037/0022-0167.44.1.53 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hiscock M., Cohen D. (1973). Visual imagery and dream recall . J. Res. Pers. 7 , 179–188 10.1016/0092-6566(73)90051-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hobson J. A. (1988). The Dreaming Brain . New York: Basic Books [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hobson J. A., McCarley R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream-state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process . Am. J. Psychiatry 134 , 1335–1348 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hobson J. A., Pace-Schott E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neuronal systems, consciousness and learning . Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3 , 679–693 10.1038/nrn915 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hobson J. A., Pace-Schott E. F., Stickgold R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states . Behav. Brain Sci. 23 , 793–842 10.1017/S0140525X00003976 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hobson J. A., Stickgold R., Pace-Schott E. F. (1998). The neuropsychology of REM sleep dreaming . Neuroreport 9 , R1–R14 10.1097/00001756-199807130-00051 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoelscher T. J., Klinger E., Barta S. G. (1981). Incorporation of concern- and nonconcern-related verbal stimuli into dream content . J. Abnorm. Psychol. 90 , 88–91 10.1037/0021-843X.90.1.88 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong C. C., Gillin J. C., Dow B. M., Wu J., Buchsbaum M. S. (1995). Localized and lateralized cerebral glucose metabolism associated with eye movements during REM sleep and wakefulness: a positron emission tomography (PET) study . Sleep 18 , 570–580 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong C. C., Harris J. C., Pearlson G. D., Kim J. S., Calhoun V. D., Fallon J. H., Golay X., Gillen J. S., Simmonds D. J., van Zijl P. C., Zee D. S., Pekar J. J. (2009). fMRI evidence for multisensory recruitment associated with rapid eye movements during sleep . Hum. Brain Mapp. 30 , 1705–1722 10.1002/hbm.20635 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jouvet M. (1991). Le sommeil paradoxal: est-il le gardien de l’individuation psychologique? Can. J. Psychol. 45 , 148–168 10.1037/h0084291 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jouvet M. (1992). Le sommeil et le rẽve . Paris: Odile Jacob [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jouvet M. (1998). Paradoxical sleep as a programming system . J. Sleep Res. 7 ( Suppl. 1 ), 1–5 10.1046/j.1365-2869.7.s1.1.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jouvet M., Michel F. (1959). Corrélations électromyographique du sommeil chez le Chat décortiqué et mésencéphalique chronique . C. R. Seances Soc. Biol. Fil. 153 , 422–425 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jouvet M., Michel F., Courjon J. (1959). Sur un stade d’activité électrique cérébrale rapide au cours du sommeil physiologique . C. R. Seances Soc. Biol. Fil. 153 , 1024–1028 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kahn D., Pace-Schott E. F., Hobson J. A. (1997). Consciousness in waking and dreaming: the roles of neuronal oscillation and neuromodulation in determining similarities and differences . Neuroscience 78 , 13–38 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00550-7 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kerr N., Foulkes D., Jurkovic G. (1978). Reported absence of visual dream imagery in a normally sighted subject with Turner’s syndrome . J. Ment. Imagery 2 , 247–264 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klimesch W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis . Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 29 , 169–195 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koechlin E., Hyafil A. (2007). Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making . Science 318 , 594–598 10.1126/science.1142995 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kosslyn S. M., Thompson W. L. (2003). When is early visual cortex activated during visual mental imagery? Psychol. Bull. 129 , 723–746 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.723 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koulack D. (1969). Effects of somatosensory stimulation on dream content . Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 20 , 718–725 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Koulack D., Goodenough D. R. (1976). Dream recall and dream recall failure: an arousal-retrieval model . Psychol. Bull. 83 , 975–984 10.1037/0033-2909.83.5.975 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kubota Y., Takasu N. N., Horita S., Kondo M., Shimizu M., Okada T., Wakamura T., Toichi M. (2011). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortical oxygenation during REM sleep in humans . Brain Res. 1389 , 83–92 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.02.061 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Legrand D., Ruby P. (2009). What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation, and critical review of neuroimaging results . Psychol. Rev. 116 , 252–282 10.1037/a0014172 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mahowald M. W., Schenck C. H. (2000). “REM sleep parasomnias,” in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine , eds Kryger M. H., Roth T., Dement W. C. (Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders; ), 724–741 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maquet P., Laureys S., Peigneux P., Fuchs S., Petiau C., Phillips C., Aerts J., Del Fiore G., Degueldre C., Meulemans T., Luxen A., Franck G., Van Der Linden M., Smith C., Cleeremans A. (2000). Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep . Nat. Neurosci. 3 , 831–836 10.1038/77744 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maquet P., Peters J., Aerts J., Delfiore G., Degueldre C., Luxen A., Franck G. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming . Nature 383 , 163–166 10.1038/383163a0 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maquet P., Ruby P., Maudoux A., Albouy G., Sterpenich V., Dang-Vu T., Desseilles M., Boly M., Perrin F., Peigneux P., Laureys S. (2005). Human cognition during REM sleep and the activity profile within frontal and parietal cortices: a reappraisal of functional neuroimaging data . Prog. Brain Res. 150 , 219–227 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50016-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Maquet P., Schwartz S., Passingham R., Frith C. (2003). Sleep-related consolidation of a visuomotor skill: brain mechanisms as assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging . J. Neurosci. 23 , 1432–1440 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Michel F., Sieroff E. (1981). Une approche anatomo-clinique des déficits de l’imagerie onirique, est-elle possible? Sleep: Proceedings of an International Colloquium . Milan: Carlo Erba Formitala [ Google Scholar ]
  • Miyauchi S., Misaki M., Kan S., Fukunaga T., Koike T. (2009). Human brain activity time-locked to rapid eye movements during REM sleep . Exp. Brain Res. 192 , 657–667 10.1007/s00221-008-1579-2 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nausieda P., Weiner W., Kaplan L., Weber S., Klawans H. (1982). Sleep disruption in the course of chronic levodopa therapy: an early feature of the levodopa psychosis . Clin. Neuropharmacol. 5 , 183–194 10.1097/00002826-198205020-00003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nielsen T. A. (2000). A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: “covert” REM sleep as a possible reconciliation of two opposing models . Behav. Brain Sci. 23 , 851–866 10.1017/S0140525X00974028 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nielsen T. A., Kuiken D., Alain G., Stenstrom P., Powell R. A. (2004). Immediate and delayed incorporations of events into dreams: further replication and implications for dream function . J. Sleep Res. 13 , 327–336 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00421.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nielsen T. A., Levrier K., Montplaisir J. (2011). Dreaming correlates of alexithymia among sleep-disordered patients . Dreaming 21 , 16–31 10.1037/a0022861 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nielsen T. A., Ouellet L., Warnes H., Cartier A., Malo J. L., Montplaisir J. (1997). Alexithymia and impoverished dream recall in asthmatic patients: evidence from self-report measures . J. Psychosom. Res. 42 , 53–59 10.1016/S0022-3999(96)00230-9 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nielsen T. A., Stenstrom P. (2005). What are the memory sources of dreaming? Nature 437 , 1286–1289 10.1038/nature04288 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nielsen T. A., Zadra A. L., Simard V., Saucier S., Stenstrom P., Smith C., Kuiken D. (2003). The typical dreams of Canadian university students . Dreaming 13 , 211–235 10.1023/B:DREM.0000003144.40929.0b [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nir Y., Tononi G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: from phenomenology to neurophysiology . Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 14 , 88–100 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nofzinger E. A., Mintun M. A., Wiseman M., Kupfer D. J., Moore R. Y. (1997). Forebrain activation in REM sleep: an FDG PET study . Brain Res. 770 , 192–201 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00807-X [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Okada H., Matsuoka K., Hatakeyama T. (2000). Dream-recall frequency and waking imagery . Percept. Mot. Skills 91 ( 3 Pt 1 ), 759–766 10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.759 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oudiette D., Leu S., Pottier M., Buzare M. A., Brion A., Arnulf I. (2009). Dreamlike mentations during sleepwalking and sleep terrors in adults . Sleep 32 , 1621–1627 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pagel J. F. (2003). Non-dreamers . Sleep Med. 4 , 235–241 10.1016/S1389-9457(02)00255-1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Palagini L., Gemignani A., Feinberg I., Guazzelli M., Campbell I. G. (2004). Mental activity after early afternoon nap awakenings in healthy subjects . Brain Res. Bull. 63 , 361–368 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.12.008 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peigneux P., Laureys S., Fuchs S., Collette F., Perrin F., Reggers J., Phillips C., Degueldre C., Del Fiore G., Aerts J., Luxen A., Maquet P. (2004). Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron 44 , 535–545 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.007 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Peigneux P., Laureys S., Fuchs S., Delbeuck X., Degueldre C., Aerts J., Delfiore G., Luxen A., Maquet P. (2001). Generation of rapid eye movements during paradoxical sleep in humans . Neuroimage 14 , 701–708 10.1006/nimg.2001.0874 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Petrides M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization . Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360 , 781–795 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Piolino P., Desgranges B., Eustache F. (2009). Episodic autobiographical memories over the course of time: cognitive, neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings . Neuropsychologia 47 , 2314–2329 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.020 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Raymond I., Nielsen T. A., Lavigne G., Choinière M. (2002). Incorporation of pain in dreams of hospitalized burn victims . Sleep 25 , 765–770 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rechtschaffen A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams . Sleep 1 , 97–109 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Revonsuo A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming . Behav. Brain Sci. 23 , 877–901; discussion 904–1121. 10.1017/S0140525X00814028 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Richardson A. (1979). Dream recall frequency and vividness of visual imagery . J. Ment. Imagery 3 , 65–72 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruby P., Collette F., D’Argembeau A., Peters F., Degueldre C., Balteau E., Luxen A., Maquet P., Salmon E. (2009). Perspective taking to assess self personality: what is modified in Alzheimer’s disease? Neurobiol. Aging 30 , 1637–1651 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.014 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruby P., Decety J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency . Nat. Neurosci. 4 , 546–550 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruby P., Decety J. (2003). What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking . Eur. J. Neurosci. 17 , 2475–2480 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02673.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruby P., Decety J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16 , 988–999 10.1162/0898929041502661 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ruby P., Schmidt C., Hogge M., D’Argembeau A., Collette F., Salmon E. (2007). Social mind representation: where does it fail in frontotemporal dementia? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19 , 1–13 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saint-Denys H. (1977). Les Rẽves et les moyens de les diriger . Paris: D’Aujourd’hui; (Originally published in 1867). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salzarulo P. (1971). Electroencephalographic and polygraphic study of afternoon sleep in normal subjects . Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 30 , 399–407 10.1016/0013-4694(71)90254-9 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sanford L. D., Cheng C. S., Silvestri A. J., Mann G. L., Morrison A. R. (2001). Sleep and behavior in rats with pontine lesions producing REM without atonia . Sleep Res. Online 4 , 1–5 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sastre J. P., Jouvet M. (1979). Le comportement onirique du chat . Physiol. Behav. 22 , 979–989 10.1016/0031-9384(79)90344-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saurat M. T., Agbakou M., Attigui P., Golmard J. L., Arnulf I. (2011). Walking dreams in congenital and acquired paraplegia . Conscious. Cogn. 20 , 1425–1432 10.1016/j.concog.2011.05.015 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Scharf B., Moskowitz C., Lupton M., Klawans H. (1978). Dream phenomena induced by chronic Levodopa therapy . J. Neural. Transm. 43 , 143–151 10.1007/BF01579073 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schonbar R. A. (1959). Some manifest characteristics of recallers and nonrecallers of dreams . J. Consult. Psychol. 23 , 414–418 10.1037/h0047400 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M. (1999). Dream recall: research, clinical implications and future directions . Sleep Hypn. 1 , 72–81 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M. (2002). Dream recall frequency and openness to experience: a negative finding . Pers. Individ. Dif. 33 , 1285–1289 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00013-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M. (2008). Dream recall frequency in a representative German sample . Percept. Mot. Skills 106 , 699–702 10.2466/pms.106.3.690-692 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M., Ciric P., Gotz S., Wittmann L. (2004). Typical dreams: stability and gender differences . J. Psychol. 138 , 485–494 10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M., Frauscher S., Shendi A. (1995). Dream recall and visual memory . Percept. Mot. Skills 81 , 256–258 10.2466/pms.1995.81.1.256 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M., Hofmann F. (2003). Continuity between waking activities and dream activities . Conscious. Cogn. 12 , 298–308 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00072-7 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M., Reinhard I. (2008). Gender differences in dream recall: a meta-analysis . J. Sleep Res. 17 , 125–131 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00626.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M., Jochum S., Souguenet S. (1997). Dream recall, visual memory, and absorption in imaginings . Pers. Individ. Dif. 2 , 291–292 10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00192-4 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schredl M., Wittmann L., Ciric P., Gotz S. (2003). Factors of home dream recall: a structural equation model . J. Sleep Res. 12 , 133–141 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00344.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz S. (1999). Exploration statistique et neuropsychologique des phénomènes oniriques au travers des textes et des images de rẽves . Ph.D. thesis, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, 375 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schwartz S., Maquet P. (2002). Sleep imaging and the neuro-psychological assessment of dreams . Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 6 , 23–30 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01818-0 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shapiro A., Goodenough D. R., Gryler R. B. (1963). Dream recall as a function of method of awakening . Psychosom. Med. 25 , 174–180 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shapiro A., Goodenough D. R., Lewis H. B., Sleser I. (1965). Gradual arousal from sleep: a determinant of thinking reports . Psychosom. Med. 27 , 342–349 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Shevrin H., Fritzler D. E. (1968). Visual evoked response correlates of unconscious mental processes . Science 161 , 295–298 10.1126/science.161.3838.295 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Solms M. (1997). The Neuropsychology of Dreaming: A Clinico-Anatomical Study . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates [ Google Scholar ]
  • Solms M. (2000). Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms . Behav. Brain Sci. 23 , 843–850 10.1017/S0140525X00964021 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stickgold R., Hobson J. A., Fosse R., Fosse M. (2001). Sleep, learning, and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing . Science 294 , 1052–1057 10.1126/science.1063530 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tart C. T. (1962). Frequency of dream recall and some personality measures . J. Consult. Psychol. 26 , 467–470 10.1037/h0046056 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson L. T., Moyer J. R., Jr., Disterhoft J. F. (1996). Transient changes in excitability of rabbit CA3 neurons with a time course appropriate to support memory consolidation . J. Neurophysiol. 76 , 1836–1849 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trull T. (2005). Clinical Psychology , 7th Edn Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth [ Google Scholar ]
  • Uga V., Lemut M. C., Zampi C., Zilli I., Salzarulo P. (2006). Music in dreams . Conscious. Cogn. 15 , 351–357 10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.003 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valli K., Frauscher B., Gschliesser V., Wolf E., Falkenstetter T., Schönwald S. V., Ehrmann L., Zangerl A., Marti I., Boesch S. M., Revonsuo A., Poewe W., Högl B. (2011). Can observers link dream content to behaviours in rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder? A cross-sectional experimental pilot study . J. Sleep Res. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00938.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valli K., Revonsuo A. (2009). The threat simulation theory in light of recent empirical evidence: a review . Am. J. Psychol. 122 , 17–38 [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Valli K., Revonsuo A., Pälkäs O., Ismail K. H., Ali K. J., Punamäki R. L. (2005). The threat simulation theory of the evolutionary function of dreaming: eidence from dreams of traumatized children . Conscious. Cogn. 14 , 188–218 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00019-9 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Voss U., Tuin I., Schermelleh-Engel K., Hobson A. (2010). Waking and dreaming: related but structurally independent. Dream reports of congenitally paraplegic and deaf-mute persons . Conscious. Cogn. 20 , 673–687 10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.020 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wagner U., Gais S., Haider H., Verleger R., Born J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight . Nature 427 , 352–355 10.1038/nature02223 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wamsley E. J., Tucker M., Payne J. D., Benavides J. A., Stickgold R. (2010). Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced sleep-dependant memory consolidation . Curr. Biol. 20 , 850–855 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.012 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wehrle R., Czisch M., Kaufmann C., Wetter T. C., Holsboer F., Auer D. P., Pollmächer T. (2005). Rapid eye movement-related brain activation in human sleep: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study . Neuroreport 16 , 853–857 10.1097/00001756-200505310-00015 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wittmann L., Palmy C., Schredl M. (2004). NREM sleep dream recall, dream report length and cortical activation . Sleep Hypn. 6 , 53–57 [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zacks J. M., Vettel J. M., Michelon P. (2003). Imagined viewer and object rotations dissociated with event-related FMRI . J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15 , 1002–1018 10.1162/089892903770007399 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

Change Password

Your password must have 6 characters or more:.

  • a lower case character, 
  • an upper case character, 
  • a special character 

Password Changed Successfully

Your password has been changed

Create your account

Forget yout password.

Enter your email address below and we will send you the reset instructions

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to reset your password

Forgot your Username?

Enter your email address below and we will send you your username

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to retrieve your username

Psychiatry Online

  • April 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 4 CURRENT ISSUE pp.255-346
  • March 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 3 pp.171-254
  • February 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 2 pp.83-170
  • January 01, 2024 | VOL. 181, NO. 1 pp.1-82

The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use , including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

The activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreams: a theoretical note

The author examines Hobson and McCarley's activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreams from the point of view of theory construction and the logic of science. After reviewing pertinent literature, he concludes that modern sciences has not yet established a well-defined mind-body isomorphism. Therefore, conclusions about the psychological meaning and motive of dreams cannot validly be drawn from neurobiological data.

Access content

  • Personal login
  • Institutional Login
  • Sign in via OpenAthens
  • Register for access

Please login/register if you wish to pair your device and check access availability.

Not a subscriber.

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5 library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

  • Psychoanalytic Dream Theory And Recent Neurobiological Findings About Rem Sleep 1 July 2016 | Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, Vol. 32, No. 4
  • Psychoanalysis, Dreaming and the Rem State: A Clinical Vignette 1 November 1981 | The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 26, No. 7

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2023 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Why Do We Dream?

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Dr. Sabrina Romanoff, PsyD, is a licensed clinical psychologist and a professor at Yeshiva University’s clinical psychology doctoral program.

activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

Verywell / Madelyn Goodnight

What Is a Dream?

The role of dreams.

  • Reflect the Unconscious
  • Process Information
  • Aid In Memory
  • Spur Creativity
  • Reflect Your Life
  • Prepare and Protect
  • Process Emotions
  • Other Theories

Lucid Dreaming

Stress dreams.

Despite scientific inquiry, we still don't have a solid answer for why people dream. Some of the most notable theories are that dreaming helps us process memories and better understand our emotions , also providing a way to express what we want or to practice facing our challenges.

7 Theories on Why We Dream

A dream includes the images, thoughts, and emotions that are experienced during sleep. Dreams can range from extraordinarily intense or emotional to very vague, fleeting, confusing, or even boring. Some dreams are joyful, while others are frightening or sad. Sometimes dreams seem to have a clear narrative, while many others appear to make no sense at all.

There are many unknowns about dreaming and sleep, but what scientists do know is that just about everyone dreams every time they sleep, for a total of around two hours per night, whether they remember it upon waking or not .

Beyond what's in a particular dream, there is the question of why we dream at all. Below, we detail the most prominent theories on the purpose of dreaming and how these explanations can be applied to specific dreams.

How Do Scientists Study Dreams?

The question of why we dream has fascinated philosophers and scientists for thousands of years. Traditionally, dream content is measured by the subjective recollections of the dreamer upon waking. However, observation is also accomplished through objective evaluation in a lab.

In one study, researchers even created a rudimentary dream content map that was able to track what people dreamed about in real time using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) patterns. The map was then backed up by the dreamers' reports upon waking.

Some of the more prominent dream theories contend that the function of dreaming is to:

  • Consolidate memories
  • Process emotions
  • Express our deepest desires
  • Gain practice confronting potential dangers

Many experts believe that we dream due to a combination of these reasons rather than any one particular theory. Additionally, while many researchers believe that dreaming is essential to mental, emotional, and physical well-being, some scientists suggest that dreams serve no real purpose at all.

The bottom line is, while many theories have been proposed, no single consensus has emerged on why we dream.

Dreaming during different phases of sleep may also serve unique purposes. The most vivid dreams happen during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep , and these are the dreams that we're most likely to recall. We also dream during non-rapid eye movement (non-REM) sleep, but those dreams are known to be remembered less often and have more mundane content.

Dreams May Reflect the Unconscious

Sigmund Freud’s theory of dreams suggests that dreams represent  unconscious desires, thoughts, wish fulfillment, and motivations. According to Freud, people are driven by repressed and unconscious longings, such as aggressive and sexual instincts .

While many of Freud's assertions have been debunked, research suggests there is a dream rebound effect, also known as dream rebound theory, in which suppression of a thought tends to result in dreaming about it.

What Causes Dreams to Happen?

In " The Interpretation of Dreams ," Freud wrote that dreams are "disguised fulfillments of repressed wishes." He also described two different components of dreams: manifest content (actual images) and latent content (hidden meaning).

Freud’s theory contributed to the rise and popularity of dream interpretation . While research has failed to demonstrate that the manifest content disguises the psychological significance of a dream, some experts believe that dreams play an important role in processing emotions and stressful experiences.

Dreams Process Information

According to the activation-synthesis model of dreaming , which was first proposed by J. Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley, circuits in the brain become activated during REM sleep, which triggers the amygdala and hippocampus to create an array of electrical impulses. This results in a compilation of random thoughts, images, and memories that appear while dreaming.

When we wake, our active minds pull together the various images and memory fragments of the dream to create a cohesive narrative.  

In the activation-synthesis hypothesis, dreams are a compilation of randomness that appear to the sleeping mind and are brought together in a meaningful way when we wake. In this sense, dreams may provoke the dreamer to make new connections, inspire useful ideas, or have creative epiphanies in their waking lives.

Dreams Aid In Memory

According to the information-processing theory, sleep allows us to consolidate and process all of the information and memories that we have collected during the previous day. Some dream experts suggest that dreaming is a byproduct, or even an active part, of this experience processing.  

This model, known as the self-organization theory of dreaming , explains that dreaming is a side effect of brain neural activity as memories are consolidated during sleep. During this process of unconscious information redistribution, it is suggested that memories are either strengthened or weakened. According to the self-organization theory of dreaming, while we dream, helpful memories are made stronger, while less useful ones fade away.

Research supports this theory, finding improvement in complex tasks when a person dreams about doing them. Studies also show that during REM sleep, low-frequency theta waves were more active in the frontal lobe, just like they are when people are learning, storing, and remembering information when awake.

Dreams Spur Creativity

Another theory about dreams says that their purpose is to help us solve problems. In this creativity theory of dreaming, the unconstrained, unconscious mind is free to wander its limitless potential while unburdened by the often stifling realities of the conscious world. In fact, research has shown dreaming to be an effective promoter of creative thinking.

Scientific research and anecdotal evidence back up the fact that many people do successfully mine their dreams for inspiration and credit their dreams for their big "aha" moments.

The ability to make unexpected connections between memories and ideas that appear in your dreams often proves to be an especially fertile ground for creativity.

Dreams Reflect Your Life

Under the continuity hypothesis, dreams function as a reflection of a person's real life, incorporating conscious experiences into their dreams. Rather than a straightforward replay of waking life, dreams show up as a patchwork of memory fragments.

Still, studies show that non-REM sleep may be more involved with declarative memory (the more routine stuff), while REM dreams include more emotional and instructive memories. In general, REM dreams tend to be easier to recall compared to non-REM dreams.

Under the continuity hypothesis, memories may be fragmented purposefully in our dreams as part of incorporating new learning and experiences into long-term memory . Still, there are many unanswered questions as to why some aspects of memories are featured more or less prominently in our dreams.

Dreams Prepare and Protect

The primitive instinct rehearsal and adaptive strategy theories of dreaming propose that we dream to better prepare ourselves to confront dangers in the real world. The dream as a social simulation function or threat simulation provides the dreamer a safe environment to practice important survival skills.

While dreaming, we hone our fight-or-flight instincts and build mental capability for handling threatening scenarios. Under the threat simulation theory, our sleeping brains focus on the fight-or-flight mechanism to prep us for life-threatening and/or emotionally intense scenarios including:

  • Running away from a pursuer
  • Falling over a cliff
  • Showing up somewhere naked
  • Going to the bathroom in public
  • Forgetting to study for a final exam

This theory suggests that practicing or rehearsing these skills in our dreams gives us an evolutionary advantage in that we can better cope with or avoid threatening scenarios in the real world. This helps explain why so many dreams contain scary, dramatic, or intense content.

Dreams Help Process Emotions

The emotional regulation dream theory says that the function of dreams is to help us process and cope with our emotions or trauma in the safe space of slumber.

Research shows that the amygdala, which is involved in processing emotions, and the hippocampus, which plays a vital role in condensing information and moving it from short-term to long-term memory storage, are active during vivid, intense dreaming. This illustrates a strong link between dreaming, memory storage, and emotional processing.

This theory suggests that REM sleep plays a vital role in emotional brain regulation. It also helps explain why so many dreams are emotionally vivid and why emotional or traumatic experiences tend to show up on repeat. Research has shown a connection between the ability to process emotions and the amount of REM sleep a person gets.

Content similarities and common dreams shared among dreamers may help promote connection. Research also notes heightened empathy among people who share their dreams with others, pointing to another way dreams can help us cope by promoting community and interpersonal support.

Other Theories About Why We Dream

Many other theories have been suggested to account for why we dream.

  • One theory contends that dreams are the result of our brains trying to interpret external stimuli (such as a dog's bark, music, or a baby's cry) during sleep.
  • Another theory uses a computer metaphor to account for dreams, noting that dreams serve to "clean up" clutter from the mind, refreshing the brain for the next day.
  • The reverse-learning theory suggests that we dream to forget. Our brains have thousands of neural connections between memories—too many to remember them all—and that dreaming is part of "pruning" those connections.
  • In the continual-activation theory, we dream to keep the brain active while we sleep, in order to keep it functioning properly.

Lucid dreams are relatively rare dreams where the dreamer has awareness of being in their dream and often has some control over the dream content. Research indicates that around 50% of people recall having had at least one lucid dream in their lifetime and just over 10% report having them two or more times per month.

It is unknown why certain people experience lucid dreams more frequently than others. While experts are unclear as to why or how lucid dreaming occurs, preliminary research signals that the prefrontal and parietal regions of the brain play a significant role.

How to Lucid Dream

Many people covet lucid dreaming and seek to experience it more often. Lucid dreaming has been compared to virtual reality and hyper-realistic video games, giving lucid dreamers the ultimate self-directed dreamscape experience.

Potential training methods for inducing lucid dreaming include cognitive training, external stimulation during sleep, and medications. While these methods may show some promise, none have been rigorously tested or shown to be effective.

A strong link has been found between lucid dreaming and highly imaginative thinking and creative output. Research has shown that lucid dreamers perform better on creative tasks than those who do not experience lucid dreaming.

Stressful experiences tend to show up with great frequency in our dreams. Stress dreams may be described as sad, scary, and nightmarish .

Experts do not fully understand how or why specific stressful content ends up in our dreams, but many point to a variety of theories, including the continuity hypothesis, adaptive strategy, and emotional regulation dream theories to explain these occurrences. Stress dreams and mental health seem to go hand-in-hand.

  • Daily stress shows up in dreams : Research has shown that those who experience greater levels of worry in their waking lives and people diagnosed with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) report higher frequency and intensity of nightmares.
  • Mental health disorders may contribute to stress dreams : Those with mental health disorders such as anxiety, bipolar disorder , and depression tend to have more distressing dreams, as well as more difficulty sleeping in general.
  • Anxiety is linked to stress dreams : Research indicates a strong connection between anxiety and stressful dream content. These dreams may be the brain's attempt to help us cope with and make sense of these stressful experiences.

A Word From Verywell

While there are many theories for why we dream, more research is needed to fully understand their purpose. Rather than assuming only one hypothesis is correct, dreams likely serve a variety of purposes.

Knowing that so much is left uncertain about why we dream, we can feel free to view our own dreams in the light that resonates best with us.

If you are concerned about your dreams and/or are having frequent nightmares, consider speaking to your doctor or consulting a sleep specialist.

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. Brain basics: Understanding sleep .

Horikawa T, Tamaki M, Miyawaki Y, Kamitani Y. Neural decoding of visual imagery during sleep . Science . 2013;340(6132):639-42. doi:10.1126/science.1234330

De Gennaro L, Cipolli C, Cherubini A, et al. Amygdala and hippocampus volumetry and diffusivity in relation to dreaming . Hum Brain Mapp . 2011;32(9):1458-70. doi:10.1002/hbm.21120

Zhang W, Guo B. Freud's dream interpretation: A different perspective based on the self-organization theory of dreaming . Front Psychol . 2018;9:1553. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01553

Wegner DM, Wenzlaff RM, Kozak M. Dream rebound: The return of suppressed thoughts in dreams . Psychol Sci . 2004;15(4):232-6. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00657.x

Hobson JA, McCarley RW. The brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process . Am J Psychiatry . 1977;134(12):1335-48. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335

Eichenlaub JB, Van Rijn E, Gaskell MG, et al. Incorporation of recent waking-life experiences in dreams correlates with frontal theta activity in REM sleep . Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci . 2018;13(6):637-647. doi:10.1093/scan/nsy041

Zhang W. A supplement to self-organization theory of dreaming .  Front Psychol . 2016;7. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00332

Rasch B, Born J. About sleep's role in memory . Physiol Rev . 2013;93(2):681-766. doi:10.1152/physrev.00032.2012

Marzano C, Ferrara M, Mauro F, et al. Recalling and forgetting dreams: Theta and alpha oscillations during sleep predict subsequent dream recall . J Neurosci . 2011;31(18):6674-83. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0412-11.2011

Llewellyn S, Desseilles M. Editorial: Do both psychopathology and creativity result from a labile wake-sleep-dream cycle? . Front Psychol . 2017;8:1824. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01824

Revonsuo A. The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming .  Behav Brain Sci . 2000;23(6):877-901. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00004015

Ruby PM. Experimental research on dreaming: State of the art and neuropsychoanalytic perspectives . Front Psychol . 2011;2:286. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00286

Gujar N, McDonald SA, Nishida M, Walker MP. A role for REM sleep in recalibrating the sensitivity of the human brain to specific emotions . Cereb Cortex . 2011;21(1):115-23. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq064

Blagrove M, Hale S, Lockheart J, Carr M, Jones A, Valli K. Testing the empathy theory of dreaming: The relationships between dream sharing and trait and state empathy . Front Psychol . 2019;10:1351. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01351

Brown DW. Crick and Mitchison’s theory of REM sleep and neural networks .  Medical Hypotheses . 1993;40(6):329-331. doi:10.1016/0306-9877(93)90212-9

Zhang J. Continual-activation theory of dreaming .  Dynamical Psychology .

Vallat R, Ruby PM. Is it a good idea to cultivate lucid dreaming? Front Psychol . 2019;10:2585. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02585

Baird B, Mota-Rolim SA, Dresler M. The cognitive neuroscience of lucid dreaming . Neurosci Biobehav Rev . 2019;100:305-323. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.008

Stumbrys T, Daunytė V. Visiting the land of dream muses: The relationship between lucid dreaming and creativity . 2018;11(2). doi:10.11588/ijodr.2018.2.48667

Rek S, Sheaves B, Freeman D. Nightmares in the general population: Identifying potential causal factors . Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol . 2017;52(9):1123-1133. doi:10.1007/s00127-017-1408-7

Sikka P, Pesonen H, Revonsuo A. Peace of mind and anxiety in the waking state are related to the affective content of dreams . Sci Rep . 2018;8(1):12762. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-30721-1

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Find Study Materials for

  • Business Studies
  • Combined Science
  • Computer Science
  • Engineering
  • English Literature
  • Environmental Science
  • Human Geography
  • Macroeconomics
  • Microeconomics
  • Social Studies
  • Browse all subjects
  • Read our Magazine

Create Study Materials

Why do we dream? Dreams can feel bizarre but also meaningful, healing or even prophetic at the same time. Many great scientific and technological inventions were inspired by dreams. Niels Bohr first saw the structure of an atom in his dream, Einstein's Theory of Relativity was inspired by a weird dream about cows, and Larry Page, who co-founded Google, was inspired to work on his invention by an anxiety dream.

Mockup Schule

Explore our app and discover over 50 million learning materials for free.

  • Activation Synthesis Theory
  • Explanations
  • StudySmarter AI
  • Textbook Solutions
  • Approaches in Psychology
  • Basic Psychology
  • Biological Bases of Behavior
  • Biopsychology
  • Careers in Psychology
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognition and Development
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Data Handling and Analysis
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Eating Behaviour
  • Emotion and Motivation
  • Famous Psychologists
  • Forensic Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • Individual Differences Psychology
  • Issues and Debates in Psychology
  • Personality in Psychology
  • Psychological Treatment
  • Relationships
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Schizophrenia
  • Scientific Foundations of Psychology
  • Scientific Investigation
  • Sensation and Perception
  • Addiction Treatment
  • Adornos Theory
  • Altered States of Consciousness
  • An Introduction To Mental Health
  • Anger Management Programmes
  • Antidepressant Medications
  • Asch Conformity Experiments
  • Autonomic Nervous System in Psychology
  • Bickman Obedience Study
  • Biological Explanation of Depression
  • Body Language
  • Brain During Sleep
  • Brain Structure
  • Brain and Neuropsychology
  • Bystander Effect
  • Caspi et al 2003
  • Characteristics of Addiction
  • Characteristics of Mental Health
  • Clinical Depression
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Collective Behaviour
  • Community Sentencing
  • Concepts of the Self
  • Crime Punishment
  • Crime and Culture
  • Criminal Psychology
  • Depression Treatment
  • Depression Vs Sadness
  • Dispositional Factors
  • Drug Abuse Vs Addiction
  • Erikson's Stages Of Development
  • Ethical Issues in Social Influence Research
  • Eye Contact
  • Fight-or-Flight Response
  • Flow States
  • Freud Wolfman Study
  • Freud's Theory of Dreaming
  • Functions of Sleep
  • Hebbs Theory
  • How Crime Is Measured
  • Human Language and Animal Communication
  • Humanistic Theory of Self
  • Identity and Free Will
  • Improving Sleep
  • Language and Thought
  • Language, Thought And Communication
  • Lateralisation
  • Localisation of Brain Function
  • Majority and Minority Influence
  • Management Of Insomnia
  • Milgrams Agency Theory
  • Nervous System
  • Neuroimaging Techniques
  • Neurological Damage On Behaviour
  • Neuropsychology
  • Non Verbal Communication
  • Non-Verbal Behaviour
  • Penfield's Study of The Interpretive Cortex
  • Personal Space
  • Personality Scales
  • Personality Types
  • Piaget vs Vygotsky
  • Prosocial Behaviour
  • Psychological Problems
  • Rehabilitation
  • Restorative Justice
  • Self Management Psychology
  • Self Report
  • Siffre Cave Study Psychology
  • Sleep Hygiene
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Sleep and Zeitgebers
  • Social Effects
  • Stages of Sleep
  • Studying the brain
  • Symptoms Of Schizophrenia
  • The James Lange Theory
  • Theories of Depression
  • Theories of Schizophrenia
  • Theories of addiction
  • Trait Theory of Personality
  • Tulving's Gold Memory Study
  • Types of crime
  • Understanding Crime
  • Social Psychology

Lerne mit deinen Freunden und bleibe auf dem richtigen Kurs mit deinen persönlichen Lernstatistiken

Nie wieder prokastinieren mit unseren Lernerinnerungen.

However, the proponents of the Activation Synthesis Theory (Hobson and McCarely, 1977) would argue that there isn't a much deeper meaning to dreams. What we experience as dreams is just a creative interpretation of the brain 's random activity during sleep.

  • First, we will discuss the activation synthesis theory in psychology, providing an activation synthesis theory definition
  • We will delve into various activation synthesis theory examples, before discussing any activation synthesis theory criticisms
  • Finally, we will cover the Freud vs activation synthesis theory thesis

Activation Synthesis Theory – psychology

With the development of neuroimaging techniques for studying the brain came new attempts to explain what dreams are. In 1977, psychiatrists Hobson and McCarley proposed their hypothesis on dreaming, based on the neurobiological activity that was observed during REM sleep .

Contrary to Freud's theory (1900), which viewed dreams as the reflection of the unconscious, the Activation Synthesis Theory , although controversial, was supported by neuroimaging evidence .

Activation Synthesis Theory, girl sitting daydreaming with a book, StudySmarter

Activation Synthesis Theory – definition

The Activation Synthesis Theory argues that dreams don't have any inherent meaning , but rather they reflect the random activation of the brain stem during REM sleep . As REM sleep is characterised by greater activity than other sleep stages, which can be compared to the amount of activity during waking hours, the theory focuses only on REM sleep .

The Activation Synthesis Theory posits that dreams are the result of our mind's attempt to make sense of the random physiological brain activity that occurs during REM sleep.

During REM sleep the brain is very active. This brain activation suggests that rich sensory information is processed during this stage, while at the same time all motor and sensory information is blocked out from the brain. This is because of REM atonia (paralysis of muscles during REM sleep) and sensory blockade .

During REM sleep the thalamus activity suggests that all sensory information from the outside is blocked out, so the brain is isolated from the outside world during sleep.

The input that travels to the brain was proposed to be limited to impulses from the body - a result of physiological processes, which occur during sleep. Hobson and McCarley (1977) argued that the cerebral cortex creates elaborate dreams to make sense of activation caused by bodily processes.

Currently, we know that sensory processing does continue during sleep, however much more intense stimuli (louder noises or brighter lights) are needed to elicit arousal and wake us up.

Activation Synthesis Theory, computer with stats, StudySmarter

There are two main components of the theory: activation and synthesis.

Activation - during REM sleep the brain appears to be very active even though it is cut off from motor and sensory stimuli. This activation was proposed to be caused by physiological processes, which occur during sleep and was termed random activation .

Synthesis - to make sense of this random activation, initiated by the brain stem, the cerebral cortex activates and compares this activity with our memories. That is how dreams arise, when we experience activation similar to a certain emotional state or activity from our past, a dream simulates it based on that memory .

Activation Synthesis Theory – examples

Let's say that the brain's activity resembles the activity associated with running and fear. Our mind can create a dream in which we are being chased to make sense of this activation.

If the brain areas associated with pain become active our cortex can associate this activity with a recent painful memory of falling out with someone important to us and turn that into a dream.

Activation Synthesis Theory – mechanism

REM sleep is initiated by the activity of the REM-ON area in the brain stem. The sensory input to the body becomes limited and muscles paralysed. Spontaneous activity in the brain stem occurs, possibly linked to other physiological processes which occur in the body at the time.

Activation Synthesis Theory, a circuit with a brain image, StudySmarter

Other brain areas become activated, for example, the limbic system (responsible for emotional responses), the sensorimotor cortex (responsible for perception and movement) and the cerebral cortex. The cerebral cortex now compares this activity with existing memories and attempts to synthesise it.

Activation Synthesis Theory – Criticism

The Activation Synthesis Theory has inspired further theories of consciousness and dreaming and contributed to the field by providing an evidence-based neurobiological perspective on why we dream.

Still, it remains controversial and has been heavily criticised. Here are the main points of criticism of the theory:

The theory is reductionist, it reduces a complex psychological phenomenon of dreaming into the biological activity of different brain areas. Just because certain brain activity is correlated with dreaming, doesn't necessarily mean that this activity causes dreams or that no other processes are involved. Is the activation a result of dreams, or the cause of dreams?

Evidence for the Activation Synthesis Theory mainly comes from neuroimaging studies of animals. Limitations associated with the accuracy of neuroimaging techniques and animal research limit the validity and generalisability of the theory.

The theory only considers dreams to occur during REM sleep. We now know that dreaming occurs continuously throughout all stages of sleep, which the theory doesn't account for.

Some research suggests that, contrary to what the Activation Synthesis Theory predicts, dreams can be initiated with the activity in the cerebral context, not the brain stem.

The Activation Synthesis Theory can also be criticised for being descriptive rather than explanatory. It explains what might be causing dreams but doesn't explain their purpose.

Freud vs Activation Synthesis Theory thesis

Freud proposed that dreams are a reflection of our unconscious desires and conflicts. Because the unconscious content of our minds tends to be frightening and distressing, we don't usually have access to it. However, dream states allow us to access the unconscious indirectly, the latent content (the deeper meaning of dreams) is disguised in a symbolic manifest content or what we can remember from the dream.

According to Freud dreams do carry an underlying meaning and provide a unique insight into our unconscious, which he believed to control 75% of our behaviour.

In contrast, the Activation Synthesis Theory doesn't support the idea that dreams are very meaningful. It proposes dreams to be a result of random brain activation during sleep, which our mind attempts to interpret based on our memories.

Activation Synthesis Theory of Dreaming (Hobson and McCarley, 1977) - Key takeaways

  • The Activation Synthesis Theory, proposed by Hobson and McCarley (1977), posits that dreams are the result of our mind's attempt to make sense of the random brain activity that occurs during REM sleep.
  • The brain is isolated from outside sensory input during REM sleep due to the sensory blockade and REM atonia. Still, it remains very active during this stage, and this activity was termed random activation.
  • The random activation, initiated by the brain stem, is synthesised by the cerebral cortex, which results in dreams.
  • The Activation Synthesis Theory is based on animal studies and was criticised for limited validity and generalisability of evidence, being overly descriptive and reductionist.
  • The theory also assumes that brain activity during REM sleep is initiated by the brain stem, which isn't always the case. Moreover, the theory doesn't account for the occurrence of dreams during non-REM sleep.

Frequently Asked Questions about Activation Synthesis Theory

--> what does the activation-synthesis theory explain.

The Activation Synthesis Theory explains how dreams may arise based on the observed neural activity during REM sleep. Dreams result from the aforementioned areas of activity, the brain's attempt to assign meaning to the random firings and activations.

--> What is an example of activation-synthesis theory?

If impulses from muscles trigger activity in the brain stem, the cortex may make sense of this activity by creating a dream about being chased.

--> What is the main idea of the activation-synthesis hypothesis?

The main idea of the Activation Synthesis hypothesis is that dreams are a result of random patterns of brain activation and don't carry any inherent meaning.

--> How does the activation-synthesis hypothesis differ from Freud?

Freud proposed that dreams are meaningful. Dreams reflect unconscious desires and conflicts disguised in symbolic dream content. In contrast, the Activation Synthesis Theory proposed that there is no underlying meaning to dreams, as they merely reflect the random brain activation, which occurs during REM sleep.

--> What is synthesized in the activation-synthesis theory?

In the Activation Synthesis Theory, the random activation in the brain stem, which occurs during REM sleep is synthesised by the cortex as it attempts to make sense of this random activation.

Test your knowledge with multiple choice flashcards

The brain is active during sleep. True or false? 

Were dreams that were recalled after waking at night only reported? 

How many researchers independently analysed the data? 

Your score:

Smart Exams

Join the StudySmarter App and learn efficiently with millions of flashcards and more!

Learn with 47 activation synthesis theory flashcards in the free studysmarter app.

Already have an account? Log in

When do dreams occur according to the Activation Synthesis Theory?

According to the Activation Synthesis Theory, dreams occur during REM sleep, when the brain is isolated from outside sensory information but remains active.

Who proposed the Activation Synthesis Theory?

The theory was proposed by Hobson and McCarley in 1977.

What causes dreams according to the Activation Synthesis Theory?

The Activation Synthesis Theory posits that dreams are the result of our mind's attempt to make sense of the random activation that occurs during REM sleep.

How is the brain isolated from outside sensory information during REM sleep?

This brain is isolated because of  REM atonia (paralysis of muscles during REM sleep) and  sensory blockade .  

What is random activation?

Activation of brain areas observed during REM sleep. Contrary to waking activity it is not caused by outside sensory stimuli.

What is synthesized in the Activation Synthesis theory?

In the Activation Synthesis Theory, the random activation in the brain stem is synthesised by the cerebral cortex.

Flashcards

of the users don't pass the Activation Synthesis Theory quiz! Will you pass the quiz?

How would you like to learn this content?

Free psychology cheat sheet!

Everything you need to know on . A perfect summary so you can easily remember everything.

Join over 22 million students in learning with our StudySmarter App

The first learning app that truly has everything you need to ace your exams in one place

  • Flashcards & Quizzes
  • AI Study Assistant
  • Study Planner
  • Smart Note-Taking

Join over 22 million students in learning with our StudySmarter App

Sign up to highlight and take notes. It’s 100% free.

This is still free to read, it's not a paywall.

You need to register to keep reading, create a free account to save this explanation..

Save explanations to your personalised space and access them anytime, anywhere!

By signing up, you agree to the Terms and Conditions and the Privacy Policy of StudySmarter.

Entdecke Lernmaterial in der StudySmarter-App

Google Popup

Essential Sources in the Scientific Study of Consciousness

Bernard J. Baars is Senior Fellow in Theoretical Neurobiology at The Neurosciences Institute, San Diego.

William P. Banks is Professor of Psychology at Pomona College and editor-in-chief of the journal Consciousness and Cognition .

The late James B. Newman was a member of the Department of Psychology at the Colorado Neurological Institute.

  • ‹ Prev
  • Next ›

57: The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process

  • Published: 2003
  • In Special Collection: CogNet
  • Permissions
  • Cite Icon Cite

J. Allan Hobson, Robert W. McCarley, 2003. "The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process", Essential Sources in the Scientific Study of Consciousness, Bernard J. Baars, William P. Banks, James B. Newman

Download citation file:

  • Ris (Zotero)
  • Reference Manager

First Page Preview

Client account, sign in via your institution, a product of the mit press, mit press direct.

  • About MIT Press Direct

Information

  • Accessibility
  • For Authors
  • For Customers
  • For Librarians
  • Direct to Open
  • Open Access
  • Media Inquiries
  • Rights and Permissions
  • For Advertisers
  • About the MIT Press
  • The MIT Press Reader
  • MIT Press Blog
  • Seasonal Catalogs
  • MIT Press Home
  • Give to the MIT Press
  • Direct Service Desk
  • Terms of Use
  • Privacy Statement
  • Crossref Member
  • COUNTER Member  
  • The MIT Press colophon is registered in the U.S. Patent and Trademark Office

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

Activation synthesis theory

  • View history

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social | Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology : Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition  - Outline Index

Activation Synthesis Theory is a neurobiological theory of dreams , put forward by Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley in 1977 , which states that dreams are a random event caused by firing of neurons in the brain. This random firing sends signals to the body's motor systems, but because of a paralysis that occurs during REM sleep , the brain is faced with a paradox . It synthesizes a narrative by drawing on memory systems in an attempt to make sense of what it has experienced.

The original 1977 theory denied that dreams have meaning or are related to our real world environments. But this point drew criticism from other dream experts. In response, in 1988 , Hobson published a revised theory acknowledging that dreams do reflect past memories, fears, hopes, and desires.

  • 1 Race and intelligence (test data)
  • 2 Pregnancy fetishism

IMAGES

  1. What Is Synthesized in the Activation Synthesis Model of Dreaming

    activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

  2. Activation Synthesis

    activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

  3. PPT

    activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

  4. PPT

    activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

  5. What Is Synthesized in the Activation Synthesis Model of Dreaming

    activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

  6. ACTIVATION-SYNTHESIS HYPOTHESIS Definition & Meaning

    activation synthesis hypothesis psychology definition

VIDEO

  1. ATP synthase action.mov

  2. What is psychosynthesis?

  3. Blotted Science

  4. Action potential II Depolarization, Repolarization, Hyperpolarization II nerve impulse generation

  5. Kinetics: Calculating Activation Energies

  6. Positive Psychology Synthesis Presentation

COMMENTS

  1. Activation Synthesis Model of Dreaming

    The activation-synthesis theory is a neurobiological explanation of why we dream. The question of why people dream has perplexed philosophers and scientists for thousands of years, but it is only fairly recently in history that researchers have been able to take a closer look at exactly what happens in the body and brain during dreaming.

  2. Activation Synthesis Theory

    The Activation Synthesis theory is the activation of specific brain regions, and its synthesis is what causes dreams. There are bursts of neural activity that stem from the brain cell through the cerebral cortex that, causes the frontal lobe to interpret those signals and give them meaning. Generally, it is well-known that dreaming comes in the ...

  3. Activation-Synthesis Theory

    The Activation-Synthesis Theory is a neurobiological way to explain the origin of dreams. The Activation-Synthesis dream theory, also called the neural activation theory states that when humans ...

  4. Activation-synthesis hypothesis

    The activation-synthesis hypothesis, proposed by Harvard University psychiatrists John Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley, is a neurobiological theory of dreams first published in the American Journal of Psychiatry in December 1977. The differences in neuronal activity of the brainstem during waking and REM sleep were observed, and the hypothesis ...

  5. Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis

    Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis Definition. The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis is a theory in psychology that seeks to explain the nature and purpose of dreams. According to this hypothesis, dreams are a result of random brain activity during the rapid eye movement (REM) stage of sleep, which is accompanied by vivid dreaming.

  6. The Science Behind Dreaming

    One prominent neurobiological theory of dreaming is the "activation-synthesis hypothesis," which states that dreams don't actually mean anything: they are merely electrical brain impulses ...

  7. AP Psychology Study Resource: Definition Of Activation Synthesis Theory

    The activation-synthesis theory is a theory based on neurobiological studies into the reasons why we dream. Since the beginning of time, people have been confused by the process of dreaming. At one point, dreams were alleged to be the chosen method of communication with people from angels or the gods. Over time, as scientific advancements were ...

  8. 6 The Activation-Synthesis Theory of Dreaming

    The most widely publicized dream theory of the late twentieth and early twenty-first centuries, the neurophysiological "activation-synthesis" theory, is in every way the opposite of the neurocognitive approach to dreaming and dream meaning presented in this book, and its advocates have been critical of content analysis and the findings on dream content.

  9. Dream theories Freud, activation synthesis hypothesis

    Sigmund Freud's theory suggests dreams represent our unconscious wishes, urges, and feelings, divided into manifest and latent content. On the other hand, the activation synthesis hypothesis proposes dreams are our brain's attempt to make sense of random signals from the brain stem. These contrasting ideas offer insights into the importance of ...

  10. The brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis

    Recent research in the neurobiology of dreaming sleep provides new evidence for possible structural and functional substrates of formal aspects of the dream process. The data suggest that dreaming sleep is physiologically determined and shaped by a brain stem neuronal mechanism that can be modeled physiologically and mathematically. Formal features of the generator processes with strong ...

  11. Working on dreams, from neuroscience to psychotherapy

    The activation-synthesis hypothesis formulated by Hobson and McCarley, in 1977, assumed that the activation of pontine structures, which induces REM sleep, stimulates the brain from within, producing information that is projected onto the forebrain and limbic system and processed by these structures to recuperate the memory, construct the story ...

  12. Activation-synthesis theory (Hobson and McCarley 1977)

    Activation synthesis. Hobson and McCarley argue that dreams occur when the mind tries to make sense of the activity in the brain which is taking place whilst someone sleeps. They also believe that the brain activity that occurs (especially during REM sleep) serves some other purpose, such as restoration and protein synthesis.

  13. ACTIVATION-SYNTHESIS HYPOTHESIS

    activation-synthesis hypothesis By N., Sam M.S. speculation which points out dreams as being an output of cortical interpretation of haphazard energizing stemming from the underside of brain tissues by U.S. psychiatrists J. Allan Hobson (193.3- ) and Robert W. McCarleyl

  14. Dreams and Dreaming

    One prominent neurobiological theory of dreaming is the activation-synthesis theory, which states that dreams don't actually mean anything. They are merely electrical brain impulses that pull random thoughts and imagery from our memories. The theory posits that humans construct dream stories after they wake up, in a natural attempt to make ...

  15. Experimental Research on Dreaming: State of the Art and

    Experimental psychology first investigated dream content and frequency. The neuroscientific approach to dreaming arose at the end of the 1950s and soon proposed a physiological substrate of dreaming: rapid eye movement sleep. ... an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. Am. J. Psychiatry 134, 1335-1348 [Google Scholar]

  16. activation-synthesis hypothesis definition

    The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis is a neurobiological theory of dreams. First proposed by Harvard University psychiatrists John Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley in 1977, the hypothesis suggests that dreams are created by changes in neuron activity that activates the brainstem during REM sleep. In other words, as the body and brain cycles ...

  17. The activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreams: a theoretical note

    The author examines Hobson and McCarley's activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreams from the point of view of theory construction and the logic of science. After reviewing pertinent literature, he concludes that modern sciences has not yet established a well-defined mind-body isomorphism. Therefore, conclusions about the psychological meaning and motive of dreams cannot validly be drawn from ...

  18. Why Do We Dream? Top Dream Theories

    In the activation-synthesis hypothesis, dreams are a compilation of randomness that appear to the sleeping mind and are brought together in a meaningful way when we wake. In this sense, dreams may provoke the dreamer to make new connections, inspire useful ideas, or have creative epiphanies in their waking lives.

  19. Activation Synthesis Theory: Definition

    The Activation Synthesis Theory, proposed by Hobson and McCarley (1977), posits that dreams are the result of our mind's attempt to make sense of the random brain activity that occurs during REM sleep. The brain is isolated from outside sensory input during REM sleep due to the sensory blockade and REM atonia.

  20. The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis

    William P. Banks is Professor of Psychology at Pomona College and editor-in-chief of the journal Consciousness and Cognition. Search for other works by this author on: ... 2003. "The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process", Essential Sources in the Scientific Study of Consciousness, Bernard J ...

  21. The activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreams: A theoretical note

    Examines J. A. Hobson and R. W. McCarley's (see PA, Vol 60:4616 and 8813) activation-synthesis hypothesis of dreams from the point of view of theory construction and the logic of science. After a review of pertinent literature, it is concluded that modern science has not yet established a well-defined mind-body isomorphism. Therefore, conclusions about the psychological meaning and motive ...

  22. Activation synthesis theory

    Activation Synthesis Theory is a neurobiological theory of dreams, put forward by Allan Hobson and Robert McCarley in 1977, which states that dreams are a random event caused by firing of neurons in the brain. This random firing sends signals to the body's motor systems, but because of a paralysis that occurs during REM sleep, the brain is ...

  23. AP Psychology Dream Theories Flashcards

    AP Psychology: Hypnosis and Drugs. Teacher 23 terms. jtkender. Preview. AP Psychology - Unit 7. Teacher 95 terms. purkiss_shane. Preview. psyc 2200 exam 2. 66 terms. Oliviaamodeo_ ... Critical considerations of Activation-synthesis theory. Dream content reflects dreamers' cognitive development-their knowledge and understanding.